• Tidak ada hasil yang ditemukan

Conclusion

Dalam dokumen THE SCIENCE OF SUBJECTIVE WELL-BEING (Halaman 131-139)

This chapter examined the structural relations among various components of subjective well-being. These components can be divided roughly into cognitive components (LS and DS) and affective components (PA and NA). This chapter revealed numerous robust structural relations among these components. DS and LS are highly correlated even after controlling for shared method effects and common influences of personality traits. At least some of this relation is due to bottom-up influences of DS on LS. Thus, changes in DS are likely to produce changes in LS. It is also possible that top-down processes contribute to the LS–

DS correlation, although evidence for top-down effects is less conclusive. On the affective side, PA and NA are separable components of subjective well-being with distinct causes, although they may not be strictly independent, especially in momentary assessments of affect. Personality traits, especially neuroticism and extraversion, appear to have a stronger influence on the affective component of subjective well-being than the cognitive component. Indeed, the relation between personality and the cognitive component of subjective well-being is often fully mediated by the affective component, presumably because people rely on the affective component to judge LS. In contrast, DS is a stronger determi-nant of LS than affective well-being. The review also revealed the need for more powerful research designs to elucidate the causal processes underlying the struc-ture of subjective being. Longitudinal studies that reveal changes in well-being, in combination with dyadic designs that allow separating personality/

genetic from situational/environmental influences, are especially promising (Nes, Roysamb, Tambs, Harris, & Reichborn-Kjennerud, 2006; Schimmack & Lucas, 2007).

References

Andrews, F. M., & Withey, S. B. (1976). Social indicators of well-being. New York: Plenum Press.

Bagby, R. M., Levitan, R. D., Kennedy, S. H., Levitt, A. J., & Joffe, R. T. (1999). Selec-tive alteration of personality in response to noradrenergic and serotonergic antide-pressant medication in depressed sample: Evidence of non-specificity. Psychiatry Research, 86, 211–216.

Bookwala, J., & Schulz, R. (1996). Spousal similarity in subjective well-being: The car-diovascular health study. Psychology and Aging, 11, 582–590.

Bradburn, N. M. (1969). The structure of psychological well being. Chicago: Aldine.

Brief, A. P., Butcher, A. H., George, J. M., & Link, K. E. (1993). Integrating bottom-up and top-down theories of subjective well-being: The case of health. Journal of Person-ality and Social Psychology, 64, 646–653.

Browne, M. W., & Cudeck, R. (1993). Alternative ways of assessing model fit. In K. A.

Bollen & J. S. Long (Eds.), Testing structural equation models (pp. 136–162). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.

Clark, L. A., & Watson, D. (1988). Mood and the mundane: Relations between daily life events and self-reported mood. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 296–

308.

Colvin, C. R., & Block, J. (1994). Do positive illusions foster mental health? An exami-nation of the Taylor and Brown formulation. Psychological Bulletin, 116, 3–20.

Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (1980). Influence of extraversion and neuroticism on sub-jective well-being: Happy and unhappy people. Journal of Personality and Social Psy-chology, 38, 668–678.

De Fruyt, F., Van Leeuwen, K., Bagby, R. M., Rolland, J.-P., & Rouillon, F. (2006).

Assessing and interpreting personality change and continuity in patients treated for major depression. Psychological Assessment, 18, 71–80.

Derryberry, D., & Reed, M. A. (1994). Temperament and attention: Orienting toward and away from positive and negative signals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-ogy, 66, 1128–1139.

Diener, E. (1984). Subjective well-being. Psychological Bulletin, 95, 542–575.

Diener, E., & Biswas-Diener, R. (2002). Will money increase subjective well-being?

Social Indicators Research, 57, 119–169.

Diener, E., Colvin, C. R., Pavot, W. G., & Allman, A. (1991). The psychic costs of intense positive affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 492–503.

Diener, E., & Diener, C. (1996). Most people are happy. Psychological Science, 7, 181–185.

Diener, E., & Emmons, R. A. (1984). The independence of positive and negative affect.

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 47, 1105–1117.

Diener, E., Emmons, R. A., Larsen, R. J., & Griffin, S. (1985). The Satisfaction with Life Scale. Journal of Personality Assessment, 49, 71–75.

Diener, E., & Iran-Nejad, A. (1986). The relationship in experience between various types of affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 1031–1038.

Diener, E., & Oishi, S. (2000). Money and happiness: Income and subjective well-being across nations. In E. Diener & E. M. Suh (Eds.), Culture and subjective well-being (pp. 185–218). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Diener, E., Smith, H., & Fujita, F. (1995). The personality structure of affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 130–141.

Diener, E., Suh, E. M., Lucas, R. E., & Smith, H. L. (1999). Subjective well-being:

Three decades of progress. Psychological Bulletin, 125, 276–302.

Diener, E., Wolsic, B., & Fujita, F. (1995). Physical attractiveness and subjective well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 120–129.

Di Tella, R., Heisken De New, J., & MacCulloch, R. (2006). Happiness adaptation to income and to status in an individual panel. Unpublished manuscript, Harvard Univer-sity, Cambridge, MA.

Du, L., Bakish, D., Ravindran, A. V., & Hrdina, P. D. (2002). Does fluoxetine influence major depression by modifying five-factor personality traits? Journal of Affective Disor-ders, 71, 235–241.

Eid, M. (1995). Modelle der Messung von Personen in Situationen [Models of measuring individ-uals in situations]. Weinheim, Germany: Beltz-PVU.

Flory, J. D., Manuck, S. B., Matthews, K. A., & Muldoon, M. F. (2004). Serotonergic function in the central nervous system is associated with daily ratings of positive mood. Psychiatry Research, 129, 11–19.

Frijda, N. H. (1993). Moods, emotion episodes, and emotions. In M. Lewis & J. M.

Haviland (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (pp. 381–403). New York: Guilford Press.

Fujita, F., & Diener, E. (2005). Life satisfaction set point: Stability and change. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88, 158–164.

Green, D. P., Goldman, S. L., & Salovey, P. (1993). Measurement error masks bipolarity in affect ratings. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 1029–1041.

Greenberg, B. D., Li, Q., Lucas, F. R., Hu, S., Sirota, L. A., Benjamin, J., et al. (2000).

Association between the serotonin transporter promoter polymorphism and person-ality traits in a primarily female population sample. American Journal of Medical Genet-ics, 96, 202–216.

Headey, B., Holmstrom, E., & Wearing, A. (1984). The impact of life events and changes in domain satisfactions on well-being. Social Indicators Research, 15, 203–227.

Headey, B., Veenhoven, R., & Wearing, A. (1991). Top-down versus bottom-up theo-ries of subjective well-being. Social Indicators Research, 24, 81–100.

Heller, D., Judge, T. A., & Watson, D. (2002). The confounding role of personality and trait affectivity in the relationship between job and life satisfaction. Journal of Organi-zational Behavior, 23, 815–835.

Heller, D., Watson, D., & Hies, R. (2004). The role of person versus situation in life sat-isfaction: A critical examination. Psychological Bulletin, 130, 574–600.

Hsieh, C. M. (2004). Income and financial satisfaction among older adults in the United States. Social Indicators Research, 66, 249–266.

Hu, L., & Bentler, P. M. (1995). Evaluating model fit. In R. H. Hoyle (Ed.), Structural equation modeling: Concepts, issues, and applications (pp. 76–99). London: Sage.

Hu, L., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling, 6, 1–55.

Hunter, P. G., Schellenberg, E. G., & Schimmack, U. (in press). Mixed moods: Affective responses to music with conflicting cues. Cognition and Emotion.

Inglehart, R. (1997). Modernization and post-modernization. Princeton, NJ: Princeton Uni-versity Press.

Inglehart, R., Basanez, M., & Diez-Medrano, J. (Eds.). (2004). Human beliefs and values: A cross-cultural sourcebook based on the 1999–2002 World Values Survey. Mexico City:

Siglo XXI Editores.

Larsen, J. T., McGraw, A. P., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2001). Can people feel happy and sad at the same time? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 684–696.

Larsen, J. T., McGraw, A. P., Mellers, B. A., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2004). The agony of victory and thrill of defeat: Mixed emotional reactions to disappointing wins and relieving losses. Psychological Science, 15, 325–330.

Lucas, R. E., Diener, E., & Suh, E. (1996). Discriminant validity of well-being measures.

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 616–628.

McCrae, R. R., Jang, K. L., Livesley, W. J., Riemann, R., & Angleitner, A. (2001).

Sources of structure: Genetic, environmental, and artifactual influences on the covariation of personality traits. Journal of Personality, 69, 511–535.

Meddis, R. (1972). Bipolar factors in mood adjective checklists. British Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 11, 178–184.

Nes, R. B., Roysamb, E., Tambs, K., Harris, J. R., & Reichborn-Kjennerud, T. (2006).

Subjective well-being: Genetic and environmental contributions to stability and change. Psychological Medicine, 36, 1033–1042.

Pytlik Zillig, L. M., Hemenover, S. H., & Dienstbier, R. A. (2002). What do we assess when we assess a Big 5 trait? A content analysis of the affective, behavioral and cog-nitive processes represented in the Big 5 personality inventories. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28, 847–858.

Reisenzein, R., & Schönpflug, W. (1992). Stumpf’s cognitive evaluative theory of emo-tion. American Psychologist, 47, 34–45.

Rorer, L. G. (1965). The great response-style myth. Psychological Bulletin, 63, 129–156.

Roysamb, E., Harris, J. R., Magnus, P., Vitterso, J., & Tambs, K. (2002). Subjective well-being: Sex-specific effects of genetic and environmental factors. Personality and Individual Differences, 32, 211–223.

Russell, J. A. (1980). A circumplex model of affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-ogy, 39, 1161–1178.

Russell, J. A., & Carroll, J. M. (1999). On the bipolarity of positive and negative affect.

Psychological Bulletin, 125, 3–30.

Schimmack, U. (2001). Pleasure, displeasure, and mixed feelings: Are semantic opposites mutually exclusive? Cognition and Emotion, 15, 81–97.

Schimmack, U. (2002). Frequency judgments of emotions: The cognitive basis of person-ality assessment. In P. Sedlmeier & T. Betsch (Eds.), ETC: Frequency processing and cognition (pp. 189–204). New York: Oxford University Press.

Schimmack, U. (2003). Affect measurement in experience sampling research. Journal of Happiness Studies, 4, 79–106.

Schimmack, U. (2005). Response latencies of pleasure and displeasure ratings: Further evidence for mixed feelings. Cognition and Emotion, 19, 671–691.

Schimmack, U. (2006a). Income, financial satisfaction, and life satisfaction: A test of top-down and bottom-up models of subjective well-being. Unpublished manuscript.

Schimmack, U. (2006b). Internal and external determinants of subjective well being:

Review and public policy implications. In Y. K. Ng & L. Sang Ho (Eds.), Happiness and public policy: Theory, case studies, and implications (pp. 67–88). Houndmills, Basingstoke, Hampshire, UK: Palgrave Macmillan.

Schimmack, U. (2006c). Measuring the affective component of subjective well-being: Comparing the discriminant and predictive validity of the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule and the Hedonic Balance Scale. Unpublished manuscript.

Schimmack, U. (2006d). The structure of subjective well-being: Personality, affect, life satisfac-tion, and domain satisfaction. Unpublished manuscript.

Schimmack, U. (2007). Methodological issues in the assessment of the affective compo-nent of subjective well being. In A. Ohn & M. van Dulmen (Eds.), Handbook of methods in positive psychology (pp. 96–110). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

Schimmack, U., Bockenholt, U., & Reisenzein, R. (2002). Response styles in affect rat-ings: Making a mountain out of a molehill. Journal of Personality Assessment, 78, 461–

483.

Schimmack, U., & Colcombe, S. J. (1999). Mixed feelings: Towards a theory of plea-sure and displeaplea-sure. Retrieved July 19, 2006, from www.erin.utoronto.ca/~w3psyuli/

msMixedFeelings.pdf

Schimmack, U., Coleman, K., & Diener, E. (2000, June). Mixed feelings in everyday life.

Paper presented at the 12th annual convention of the American Psychological Soci-ety, Miami.

Schimmack, U., & Crites, S. L. J. R. (2005). The structure of affect. In D. Albarracin, B.

T. Johnson, & M. P. Zanna (Eds.), The handbook of attitudes (pp. 397–435). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

Schimmack, U., & Diener, E. (1997). Affect intensity: Separating intensity and frequency in repeatedly measured affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 1313–

1329.

Schimmack, U., Diener, E., & Oishi, S. (2002). Life satisfaction is a momentary judgment and a stable personality characteristic: The use of chronically accessible and stable sources. Journal of Personality, 70, 345–384.

Schimmack, U., Diener, E., Oishi, S., & Suh, E. (2006). The influence of shared method vari-ance on the relation between life satisfaction and domain satisfaction. Unpublished manu-script.

Schimmack, U., & Lucas, R. E. (2007). Marriage matters: Spousal similarity in life satis-faction. Journal of Applied Social Science Studies, 127, 1–7.

Schimmack, U., & Oishi, S. (2005). The influence of chronically and temporarily accessi-ble information on life satisfaction judgments. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-ogy, 89, 395–406.

Schimmack, U., Oishi, S., & Diener, E. (2002). Cultural influences on the relation between pleasant emotions and unpleasant emotions: Asian dialectic philosophies or individualism–collectivism? Cognition and Emotion, 16, 705–719.

Schimmack, U., Oishi, S., Diener, E., & Suh, E. (2000). Facets of affective experiences:

A framework for investigations of trait affect. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26, 655–668.

Schimmack, U., Oishi, S., Furr, R. M., & Funder, D. C. (2004). Personality and life sat-isfaction: A facet-level analysis. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30, 1062–

1075.

Schimmack, U., Pinkus, R. T., & Lockwood, P. (2006). Personality and life satisfaction: A dyadic multimethod–occasion–construct study. Manuscript submitted for publication.

Schimmack, U., Radhakrishnan, P., Oishi, S., Dzokoto, V., & Ahadi, S. (2002). Culture, personality, and subjective well-being: Integrating process models of life satisfaction.

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82, 582–593.

Schimmack, U., & Reisenzein, R. (2002). Experiencing activation: Energetic arousal and tense arousal are not mixtures of valence and activation. Emotion, 2, 412–417.

Schimmack, U., & Sidhu, R. (2006). Global evaluative biases and well-being. Manuscript submitted for publication.

Schwarz, N., & Strack, F. (1999). Reports of subjective well-being: Judgmental processes and their methodological implications. In D. Kahneman, E. Diener, & N. Schwarz (Eds.), Well-being: The foundations of hedonic psychology (pp. 61–84). New York: Sage.

Scollon, C. N., Diener, E., Oishi, S., & Biswas-Diener, R. (2005). An experience sam-pling and cross-cultural investigation of the relation between pleasant and unpleasant affect. Cognition and Emotion, 19, 27–52.

Spotts, E. L., Neiderhiser, J. M., Towers, H., Hansson, K., Lichtenstein, P., Cederblad, M., et al. (2004). Genetic and environmental influences on marital relationships.

Journal of Family Psychology, 18, 107–119.

Suh, E., Diener, E., Oishi, S., & Triandis, H. C. (1998). The shifting basis of life satisfac-tion judgments across cultures: Emosatisfac-tions versus norms. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 482–493.

Taylor, S. E., & Brown, J. D. (1988). Illusion and well-being: A social psychological per-spective on mental health. Psychological Bulletin, 103, 193–210.

Warr, P. B., Barter, J., & Brownbridge, G. (1983). On the independence of positive and negative affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44, 644–651.

Watson, D. (1988). The vicissitudes of mood measurement: Effects of varying descriptors, time frames, and response formats on measures of positive and negative affect. Jour-nal of PersoJour-nality and Social Psychology, 55, 128–141.

Watson, D. (2000). Mood and temperament. New York: Guilford Press.

Watson, D., & Clark, L. A. (1992). Affects separable and inseparable: On the hierarchical arrangement of the negative affects. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62, 489–505.

Watson, D., & Clark, L. A. (1997). Measurement and mismeasurement of mood: Recur-rent and emergent issues. Journal of Personality Assessment, 68, 267–296.

Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 1063–1070.

Watson, D., Wiese, D., Vaidya, J., & Tellegen, A. (1999). The two general activation systems of affect: Structural findings, evolutionary considerations, and psycho-biological evidence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 820–838.

7

The Assessment

of Subjective Well-Being

Successes and Shortfalls

W

ILLIAM

P

AVOT

Research on happiness, or subjective well-being, has emerged as an important area of focus for psychology in the new millennium. A significant and growing number of researchers have broken away from the field’s preoccupation with depression, anxiety, and disorder (Myers, 2000), choosing instead to devote their attention to “positive psychology” (Seligman & Csikszentmihalyi, 2000, p. 5). As interest in subjective well-being and related constructs has grown, so too has the demand for instruments and methodologies designed to assess these constructs.

To a large extent, progress in the development of valid measures and meth-odologies has kept pace with the increases in demand. Relative to the early days of subjective well-being research (Diener, 1984), the array of measures and research methods available to contemporary researchers is impressive. The sophistication and reliability of subjective well-being measures has increased sub-stantially. Innovative methodologies now allow researchers to develop multi-method designs that overcome many of the shortcoming and weaknesses of ear-lier single-method studies.

In the midst of a successful period of development, however, some shortfalls must be noted as well. Despite the substantial advances in subjective well-being

124

assessment, the development of a broadly based, consistent, and comprehensive empirical database of subjective well-being findings has been realized only par-tially. A number of factors, such as an overreliance on single-method, cross-sectional designs and the use of narrow measures that provide only a partial assessment of subjective well-being, have combined to limit the generality of the findings from individual studies.

In the initial sections of this chapter, general issues related to the definition and assessment of subjective well-being are discussed and examples of current measures of subjective well-being are reviewed. Later sections of the chapter focus on alternative measures and methodologies, the limitations of the current subjective well-being database, and a discussion of the future issues and directions of subjective well-being research.

Dalam dokumen THE SCIENCE OF SUBJECTIVE WELL-BEING (Halaman 131-139)