• Tidak ada hasil yang ditemukan

Directory UMM :Data Elmu:jurnal:B:Biological Psichatry:Vol48.Issue3.2000:

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2017

Membagikan "Directory UMM :Data Elmu:jurnal:B:Biological Psichatry:Vol48.Issue3.2000:"

Copied!
7
0
0

Teks penuh

(1)

Attentional Dysfunction: Adults with Schizophrenia,

ADHD, and a Normal Comparison Group

Randal G. Ross, Ann Olincy, Josette G. Harris, Bernadette Sullivan,

and Allen Radant

Background: Smooth pursuit eye movement (SPEM)

ab-normalities are found in schizophrenia. These deficits

often are explained in the context of the attentional and

inhibitory deficits central to schizophrenia

psychopathol-ogy. It remains unclear, however, whether these

attention-associated eye movement abnormalities are specific to

schizophrenia or are a nonspecific expression of

atten-tional deficits found in many psychiatric disorders. Adult

attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is an

al-ternative disorder with chronic attentional and inhibitory

dysfunction. Thus, a comparison of SPEM in adult

schizo-phrenia and adult ADHD will help assess the specificity

question.

Methods: SPEM is recorded during a 16.7° per second

constant velocity task in 17 adults with ADHD, 49 adults

with schizophrenia, and 37 normal adults; all groups

included individuals between ages 25–50 years.

Results: Smooth pursuit gain and the frequency of

antic-ipatory and leading saccades are worse in schizophrenic

subjects, with normal and ADHD subjects showing no

differences on these variables.

Conclusions: Many attention-associated SPEM

abnor-malities are not present in most subjects with ADHD,

supporting the specificity of these findings to the

atten-tional deficits seen in schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry

2000;48:197–203 ©

2000

Society

of

Biological

Psychiatry

Key Words: Schizophrenia,

attention-deficit/hyperactiv-ity disorder, eye movements, smooth pursuit, saccades

Introduction

M

ore than 90 years ago, Diefendorf and Dodge

(Diefendorf and Dodge 1908) first reported that

individuals with schizophrenia had difficulty tracking a

predictably moving object. Abnormal eye tracking, one of

the most frequently studied and consistently reproduced

psychophysiologic abnormalities associated with

schizo-phrenia (Levy et al 1993), may help refine the

neurophys-iologic description of schizophrenia (Ross et al 1998b) and

facilitate genetic studies of schizophrenia (Clementz

1998). Ocular tracking of a moving target, commonly

referred to as smooth pursuit eye movements (SPEM),

requires the coordinated activation of both smooth pursuit

and saccadic systems (Gaymard and Pierrot-Deseilligny

1999). The roles of each of these eye-movement

abnor-malities in producing the eye-tracking abnorabnor-malities in

schizophrenia have yet to be fully determined.

SPEM abnormalities often can be at least partially

normalized

with

attention

enhancement

techniques

(Rosenberg et al 1997), and at least one of the pathologic

components of SPEM performance, task-inappropriate

intrusion of anticipatory saccades, has been

conceptual-ized as a failure of inhibitory control (Friedman et al 1992;

Rosenberg et al 1997; Ross et al 1996; Strik et al 1992).

Attentional and inhibitory control subjects thus appear to

play a major role in the SPEM abnormalities seen in

schizophrenia. Despite this focus on the interplay between

attentional and inhibitory processes, there has been only

minimal exploration of SPEM in another chronic

psychi-atric disorder of attentional and inhibitory dysfunction—

attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). A

pri-mary deficit in ADHD is a failure in response inhibition

(Barkley 1997), a deficit that can be identified in saccadic

eye-movement tasks (Ross et al 1994) in a manner similar

to that found in schizophrenia (Ross et al 1998b). If SPEM

abnormalities associated with schizophrenia are due to

nonspecific attentional dysfunction, individuals with

ADHD should demonstrate similar SPEM abnormalities.

Reports have been contradictory as to whether

perfor-From the Departments of Psychiatry of the Denver Veterans Administration Medical Center (RGR, AO) and University of Colorado Health Sciences Center (RGR, AO, JGH, BS), Denver, Colorado, and the Seattle Veterans Adminis-tration Medical Center, Seattle, Washington (AR).

Address reprint requests to Randal G. Ross, M.D., University of Colorado Health Sciences Center, Box C268-71, 4200 E. 9th Ave., Denver CO 80262. Received September 20, 1999; revised February 3, 2000; accepted February 3,

2000.

© 2000 Society of Biological Psychiatry 0006-3223/00/$20.00

(2)

mance during a SPEM task is abnormal in children with

ADHD (Bala et al 1981; Bylsma and Pivik 1989; Jacobsen

et al 1996; Shapira et al 1980). There is no published

literature on SPEM performance in adult ADHD and no

published reports that include information on anticipatory

saccades in ADHD individuals of any age. This study

compares adults with schizophrenia, adults with ADHD,

and a comparison group of normal subjects on a smooth

pursuit eye movement task.

Methods and Materials

Subjects

Because normal aging can impact SPEM performance (Hutton et al 1993; Kanayama et al 1994; Kuechenmeister et al 1977; Larsby et al 1988; Ross et al 1999a; Sharpe and Sylvester 1978; Spooner et al 1980), all subjects were restricted to individuals between ages 25 and 50 years. Seventeen adult ADHD subjects were recruited from an adult outpatient clinic for ADHD. All subjects were recruited early in the diagnostic process. None had received stimulant medications for at least 10 years before enrollment in this study. All subjects were assessed using DSM-IV criteria (American Psychiatric Association 1994) via the structured clinical interview for DSM-IV (First et al 1997) and a semistructured clinical interview for ADHD developed by one of the authors of our study (RR). Exclusionary criteria included history of significant head injury, neurologic illness (e.g., seizures), substance abuse or dependence within the last 6 months, current major depression, any history of psychosis or bipolar disorder, or family history of psychosis or mania. ADHD subjects were identified as currently positive on 8.3 6 1.0 inattentive symptoms (range 6 –9) and 5.5 62.4 hyperactive-impulsive symptoms (range 2– 8). Six subjects (4 men and 2 women) were diagnosed with ADHD-combined subtype; 5 subjects (3 men and 2 women) were diagnosed with ADHD-inattentive subtype. All subjects had onset of symptoms before age 7 years. Wender Utah self-report scores (Ward et al 1993) were 55 6 16 with a range 37– 80 (all values greater than 2 standard deviations above reported values for gender-matched normal subjects). Four subjects (3 men and 1 woman) were diagnosed with concurrent social phobia. Past histories of psy-chiatric disorders in addition to ADHD were identified: major depression (n 5 3), obsessive-compulsive disorder (n 5 2), alcohol dependence (n 5 1), and marijuana abuse (n 5 1).

SPEM recordings from all age-appropriate schizophrenic and normal adults who have been recorded in our lab in the last 6 years were included as comparison groups. Normal adults were

excluded for history of significant head injury, neurologic illness (e.g., seizures), substance abuse or dependence within the last 6 months, current major depression, any history of psychosis or bipolar disorder, or family history of psychosis or mania. Adults with schizophrenia were recruited from outpatient psychiatric clinics. Diagnosis was confirmed with the use of the structured clinical interview for DSM-IV (First et al 1997). The recruitment process identified individuals with chronic schizophrenia who were maintained with stable symptoms on an outpatient basis. Schizophrenic subjects were excluded for neurologic illness (e.g., seizures), substance abuse or dependence within the last 6 months, and current major depression. This process produced SPEM recordings from 49 schizophrenic and 37 normal subjects for analysis. We have previously reported on many of these subjects (Ross et al 1998b, 1999a, 1999b).

ADHD, schizophrenic, and normal groups did not differ on age; however, there was a significant difference in gender (Table 1). No male-female differences exist on any eye-movement variable reported in this study.

Smooth Pursuit Eye Movements

Subjects were seated 43 cm in front of a video monitor on which a small target was displayed against a black background in an otherwise dark room. The subjects’ heads were stabilized with a bite bar and head rest. Horizontal eye movements were recorded using an infrared photoelectric limbus detection eye-tracking device, which is accurate to within 0.25° of visual angle and has a time constant of 4 msec. The analog output of the device was sampled at 500 Hz using a 12-bit analog-to-digital converter. There are no reported data assessing left-right differences in the dependent eye-movement measures employed in this study, but because disconjugate gaze is likely absent in most subjects, both eyes should be tracking the target in a similar manner. Our experience suggests that minimizing calibration time decreases the time subjects must maintain head position and improves the quality of the recording. Therefore, data were collected only from the eye for which the most rapid and accurate calibration could be obtained.

For the smooth pursuit eye-movement task, the target moved horizontally back and forth over 30° with a constant velocity of 16.7° per sec and a 1.4-sec fixation period between ramps, a “trapezoidal pattern.” Subjects were told to “keep your eyes on the target wherever it goes.”

All eye-movement data were analyzed with a computerized pattern-recognition program, and computer-analyzed results were confirmed with visual inspection by an experienced eye-movement evaluator (RR). Software-generated dependent mea-Table 1. Demographics for Adults with Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD), Schizophrenia, and a Normal

Comparison Group

ADHD group

Schizophrenic group

Normal

group Statistic Probability

N 17 49 37

Male/female 10/7 36/13 15/22 x259.5 .009

(3)

sures have excellent test-retest reliability (Roy-Byrne et al 1995). This analysis system has been described elsewhere (Radant and Hommer 1992; Ross et al 1996) and will be described briefly here. Raw data consist of eye position and target position for each 2 msec of recorded tracking. Eye movements were divided into discrete segments, and then each segment was classified as saccade, smooth pursuit, or artifact. Saccades were identified on the basis of peak velocity (greater than 35°/sec), initial acceler-ation (greater than 2000°/sec2

), and duration ($9 msec). Seg-ments not meeting velocity and acceleration criteria were con-sidered smooth pursuit or fixation. Artifactual segments caused by eye blinks and head movements show distinct morphology and were removed from the analysis by the pattern recognition software. Gain for a given interval of smooth pursuit was defined as mean eye velocity divided by target velocity. Global smooth pursuit gain was defined as the mean gain, weighted for time, of all intervals of true smooth pursuit (Abel et al 1991). Intervals defined as saccades are not included in computing smooth pursuit gain. During trapezoidal tasks, eye movements during fixation or within 250 msec of a change in target motion were discarded from the analysis because these movements may not represent normal pursuit (Lisberger and Pavelko 1989).

Lowered smooth pursuit gain can occur secondary to either impairment in the smooth pursuit system or as a compensatory mechanism to task-inappropriate saccades that intrude on other-wise normal pursuit (Abel and Ziegler 1988; Clementz and Sweeney 1990). Thus, it has been suggested that smooth pursuit gain is a global measure of task performance (Radant and Hommer 1992).

Catch-up saccades were used as the dependent measure of smooth pursuit system performance. Catch-up saccades function to significantly reduce error between foveal gaze and target location and compensate for poor smooth pursuit system perfor-mance (eye velocity below that of target velocity). Saccades, which are in the same direction as target motion and begin and end behind the target, are defined as catch-up saccades. In addition, saccades that are in the same direction as target motion but that begin behind target location and end ahead of target location are also classified as catch-up saccades if postsaccadic position error is#50% of presaccadic position error (i.e., the saccade functions to dramatically decrease the mismatch be-tween visual gaze and target location).

One type of task-inappropriate saccade, which intrudes on otherwise normal smooth pursuit, is the anticipatory saccade. Anticipatory saccades must 1) be in the direction of target motion, 2) either begin and end ahead of target location or increase position error by 100%, and 3) be followed by a 50 msec interval of eye velocity less than 50% of target velocity. This definition of anticipatory saccades is based on analyses of parameters that maximize differences between schizophrenic subjects and normal subjects (Ross et al 1999b, in press). Many authors also include a minimum amplitude criterion when defin-ing anticipatory saccades, generally greater than 4 –5° (Abel and Ziegler 1988; Clementz and Sweeney 1990). We traditionally have not included an amplitude criterion in our definition of anticipatory saccades and shall continue in this article to include anticipatory saccades of all amplitudes within the definition of anticipatory saccades. To allow comparison with other accounts,

however, we also shall subdivide anticipatory saccades into large anticipatory saccades (with amplitudes greater than 4°) and leading saccades (Ross et al 1999b).

Smooth pursuit, catch-up saccades, and anticipatory saccades account for more than 90% of eye movements during smooth pursuit tracking (Litman et al 1994; Radant and Hommer 1992). Other components (e.g., square wave jerks, backup saccades, etc.) constitute only a small portion of smooth pursuit tracking and generally do not differ between schizophrenic subjects and control subjects (Litman et al 1994; Radant and Hommer 1992). Some saccades begin behind the target, end in front of the target, and do not dramatically increase or decrease position error. These saccades do not meet the definition for either catch-up or anticipatory saccades and are not considered here.

Saccades generally are reported as frequency measures, that is, the number of each saccadic subtype divided by artifact-free recording time. The problem with this approach is that frequency measures do not include information about the amplitudes of saccades. A single large saccade may have a similar effect on global tracking as several small saccades will have. In addition to frequency data, we report the percentage of total eye movements accounted for by each saccadic subtype, a measure mathemati-cally identical to multiplying frequency by mean amplitude. Thus, the percentage of total eye movements accounted for by each saccadic subtype includes amplitude information along with frequency information in the same dependent measure (Olincy et al 1998; Ross et al 1993, 1996, 1997, 1998b). Because amplitude criteria are included in the definition of large anticipatory and leading saccades, these two variables shall only be reported as frequency variables.

Statistical Analysis

Comparisons between groups were made with an analysis of variance (ANOVA), with post hoc least-squared differences between groups. In addition, effect sizes (Cohen 1988) for comparisons between the schizophrenic and normal groups, the schizophrenic and ADHD groups, and the ADHD and normal groups were computed for each eye-movement variable. Effect sizes of 0.2– 0.49 were considered small, 0.5– 0.79 moderate, and greater than or equal to 0.8 large. Differences in effect size of greater than 0.2 were considered notable.

Results

Table 2 shows results for each eye-movement variable. A

significant effect of group was found for each

eye-movement measure, with post hoc analyses demonstrating

differences between the schizophrenic and normal groups

on all measures. Significant differences between

schizo-phrenic and ADHD groups were found in gain and the

frequencies of anticipatory and leading saccades.

Signifi-cant differences between the ADHD and normal groups

were only identified for the percentage of total eye

movements due to smooth pursuit.

(4)

saccades and the frequency of large anticipatory saccades)

to large (all other SPEM measures) effect sizes. The

largest effect sizes were for frequency of anticipatory

saccades (1.53), frequency of leading saccades (1.46), gain

(1.24), and the percentage of total eye movements due to

smooth pursuit (1.11).

Five comparisons between the schizophrenic and

ADHD groups produced effect sizes that would be

con-sidered small (

,

0.5) or below: frequency of catch-up

saccades, percentage of total eye movements due to

smooth pursuit, percentage of total eye movements due to

catch-up saccades, percentage of total eye movements due

to anticipatory saccades, and frequency of large

anticipa-tory saccades. Moderate effect sizes were produced for

smooth pursuit gain. The frequency of anticipatory

sac-cades and the frequency of leading sacsac-cades produced

large effect sizes for comparisons between the

schizo-phrenic and ADHD groups.

With one exception, effect sizes were notably lower

(difference in effect size

.

0.2) for schizophrenic and ADHD

group comparisons than for the schizophrenic and normal

group comparisons. The one exception is frequency of

leading saccades, which produced similar large effect sizes

(1.31–1.46) for comparisons between both the schizophrenic

and normal groups and the schizophrenic and ADHD groups.

Discussion

Schizophrenic adults performed more poorly than did

normal adults on each of the SPEM measures examined.

This sample contains some individuals who have been

discussed in previous reports and thus cannot be

consid-ered a replication; however, this is the largest sample of

schizophrenic subjects and normal subjects we have

pub-lished in this age range, suggesting these differences

remain even within larger groups. Additionally, the broad

range of eye-movement deficits is consistent with what

has been reported elsewhere (Clementz and Sweeney

1990; Litman et al 1994; Radant and Hommer 1992;

Sweeney et al 1994).

Impaired motion perception (Chen et al 1999a, 1999b)

and impaired ability to match eye velocity to target

velocity (Ross et al 1996; Farber et al 1997) may

contrib-ute to impaired SPEM abnormalities in schizophrenia;

however, a major contributor to schizophrenia-associated

SPEM abnormalities is, by experimental and theoretical

argument, associated with attentional deficits. One focus

of this study was to determine whether these attentional

findings are associated with the specific types of

atten-tional deficits found in schizophrenia or with nonspecific

attentional deficits that might be present in any adult with

attentional and inhibitory deficits. Adults with ADHD

were chosen as a comparison group with chronic deficits

in these areas.

Three eye-movement measures were significantly more

abnormal among the schizophrenic patients than among

the ADHD patients: gain, frequency of anticipatory

sac-cades, and frequency of leading saccades. The ADHD

group did not differ from the normal group on any of these

three measures. This suggests that the general attentional

Table 2. Smooth Pursuit Performance in Adults with Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD), Schizophrenia, and a Normal Comparison Group

ADHD group

Schizophrenic group

Normal

group F(2,100) Probability Post

hoca d (S–N)b d (S–A)b d (A–N)b

Global performance

Gain .9236.112 .8296.139 .9686.061 16.7 .000001 N,A.S 1.24 0.71 0.56 % due to smooth pursuitc 70.4610.7 64.1615.4 78.267.8 13.9 .00005 N.A,S 1.11 0.43 0.89

Smooth pursuit performance

Catch-up saccades (#/min)d 59.0621.8 71.8633.4 50.5621.5 6.3 .003 S.N 0.74 0.41 0.40

% due to catch-up saccadesc

11.564.0 13.665.9 8.863.5 10.2 .00009 S.N 0.96 0.38 0.74

Amplitude catch-up saccades

2.560.9 2.360.6 2.160.5 2.4 .10 — 0.36 0.30 0.62

Anticipatory saccades (ant sac)

Ant sac (#/min)d 11.6612.8 31.9620.0 8.066.3 29.0 .000000 S.N,A 1.53 1.10 0.41

% due to ant sacc 7.9615.4 10.5610.5 3.164.5 5.9 .004 S.N 0.87 0.22 0.52

Specialized saccades (#/min)d

Leading saccades 4.564.9 22.5615.6 4.864.1 32.1 .000000 S.N,A 1.46 1.31 0.07 Large ant sac 6.6611.9 7.8610.3 3.065.0 3.0 .05 S.N 0.57 0.11 0.46

All values are means6SD.

aPost hoc tests are least-squared differences: S, schizophrenia, A, ADHD, N, normal.

bEffect size (d) as defined by Cohen (1988): S-N, Schizophrenic group versus normal group comparison; S-A, schizophrenic group versus ADHD group comparison;

A-N, ADHD group versus normal group comparison.

(5)

and inhibitory dysfunction associated with ADHD does

not translate into major SPEM abnormalities in the

major-ity of ADHD patients. Nonetheless, for two of these three

measures (gain and frequency of anticipatory saccades),

the effect sizes are notably larger for the comparisons

between the schizophrenic and normal groups than for

comparisons between the schizophrenic and ADHD

groups. This is primarily attributable to a subgroup of

ADHD subjects who performed abnormally (for example,

see results for gain in Figure 1). Heterogeneity of eye

movement performance also has been found among

schizophrenic subjects (Holzman et al 1984, 1988).

One SPEM measure, the frequency of leading saccades,

produced similar effect sizes for both

schizophrenic-normal comparisons (1.46) and schizophrenic-ADHD

comparisons (1.31). Leading saccades are a subset of

anticipatory saccades. While tracking a moving object,

there is a natural tendency to generate a saccade and move

the eye ahead of current target location. For example,

while watching a car skid across an icy intersection, an

individual frequently will generate a saccade along the

projected path of the car to determine if and what the car

might hit. These anticipations of car movement are termed

“anticipatory saccades.” The target tracking task employed

in this study is simple and highly predictable. As phase lag

during this task approaches zero, this task elicits predictive

smooth pursuit. Anticipatory saccades, although rare in

normal adult subjects, do occur during this task. This

suggests that even in this simple task, saccadic anticipation

of target motion is a normal process. The SPEM tasks used

in the study of schizophrenia includes the instruction to

“keep your eye on the target,” thus requiring the subject to

inhibit the tendency to use saccades to anticipate target

motion. The failure to inhibit anticipatory saccades has

been proposed as a marker of genetic risk for

schizophre-nia because it is present in adults with schizophreschizophre-nia and

their relatives (Whicker et al 1985; Rosenberg et al 1997;

Ross et al 1998b). The lack of an increase in anticipatory

saccades in relatives of depressed patients (Rosenberg et al

1997) has argued for the possible specificity of this

marker. One of the major problems on research with this

marker, however, has been some disagreement on whether

amplitude criteria should be included in the definition.

Although a number of authors have argued for a minimum

amplitude criteria of 4 –5° (Abel et al 1991; Clementz and

Sweeney 1990), other labs have suggested that there is no

a priori reason to assume that anticipatory saccades cannot

include saccades of smaller amplitudes (Radant and

Hom-mer 1992; Ross et al 1999b). Fortunately, Strik et al

(1992) provided a possible solution to this issue when they

classified saccades solely based on amplitude criteria; they

found that saccades less than 4° were closely related to

another pathophysiologic dysfunction characterized in

schizophrenia: reduced amplitude of the auditory N100

event-related potential. They argued that larger saccades

represented a nonspecific attentional dysfunction. This

finding raised the possibility that saccades with amplitudes

greater than 4° represent a different physiologic process

than those with amplitudes less than 4°. The results from

the current study support the application of this hypothesis

to anticipatory saccades. Large anticipatory saccades

(am-plitudes

.

4°) occur significantly more often in

schizo-phrenic subjects than in normal subjects, replicating

pre-vious work (Litman et al 1994; Rosenberg et al 1997);

however, they do not differ between schizophrenic

sub-jects and ADHD subsub-jects, and the schizophrenic-ADHD

effect size, at 0.11, is below what is considered to be even

a small physiologic effect (Cohen 1998). Thus, as

pre-dicted by Strik et al (1992), the frequency of large

anticipatory saccades appears to be a nonspecific effect of

attentional dysfunction. Conversely, leading anticipatory

saccades (anticipatory saccades with amplitudes 1– 4°) are

significantly elevated in schizophrenia relative to both

ADHD and normal subjects, and there appears to be no

notable difference between ADHD subjects and normal

subjects (effect size 0.07, see Figure 2) on this variable.

For leading saccades, the effect size is similar in

schizo-phrenic-normal comparisons and schizophrenic-ADHD

comparisons, supporting the specificity of this finding to

schizophrenia.

In summary, as a group, ADHD subjects did not

significantly differ from normal subjects on the majority

of SPEM measures used in this study. Nonetheless, for

most measures, a few ADHD subjects performed very

poorly. ADHD subjects were identified based on a

(6)

structured evaluation by a single investigator (RGR),

without interrater reliability assessment; however, subjects

identified as meeting criteria for ADHD also scored poorly

on the Wender self-report scale, a scale associated with

ADHD in adults (Ward et al 1993), suggesting that a

majority of these subjects likely had ADHD. Additionally,

ADHD subjects and schizophrenic subjects were not

matched for severity of attentional dysfunction, and it can

be assumed that, on average, the schizophrenic subjects

demonstrated more severe psychopathology. The

gener-ally lower level of psychopathology in the ADHD subjects

(relative to the schizophrenic subjects) cannot be entirely

ruled out as a contributor to the generally normal range of

SPEM performance exhibited by the ADHD subjects.

Nonetheless, nonpsychotic relatives of schizophrenic

pro-bands also show SPEM abnormalities, and in particular

anticipatory saccade abnormalities, similar to the

psy-chotic probands (Whicker et al 1985; Ross et al 1998b).

Thus, SPEM and anticipatory saccade abnormalities

ap-pear to reflect genetic vulnerability rather than severity of

disease.

One of the goals of determining the specificity of SPEM

abnormalities to schizophrenia is assessing the possible

utility of SPEM abnormalities as a genetic risk marker in

family linkage studies. ADHD is a common disorder in the

population (Biederman 1998) and is likely to occur in

many families used for linkage analyses. The present

results suggest that the percentage of leading saccades is

unlikely to be elevated as the result of general attentional

dysfunction, thus allowing the inclusion of ADHD family

members in genetic studies.

This research was funded by a NARSAD Junior Investigator Award (RGR), by The Veterans Administration Medical Research Service, and by USPHS Grants Nos. MH56539, MH44212, and MH38321.

References

Abel L, Friedman L, Jesberger J, Malki A, Meltzer HY (1991): Quantitative assessment of smooth pursuit gain and catch-up saccades in schizophrenia and affective disorders. Biol

Psy-chiatry 29:1063–1072.

Abel L, Ziegler A (1988): Smooth pursuit eye movements in schizophrenics—what constitutes quantitative assessment?

Biol Psychiatry 24:747–761.

American Psychiatric Association (1994): Diagnostic and

Sta-tistical Manual of Mental Disorders, (4th ed). Washington,

DC: American Psychiatric Association Press.

Bala SP, Cohen B, Morris AG, Atkin A, Gittelman R, Kates W (1981): Saccades of hyperactive and normal boys during ocular pursuit. Dev Med Child Neurol 23:323–336. Barkley RA (1997): Behavioral inhibition, sustained attention,

and executive functions: Constructing a unifying theory of ADHD. Psychol Bull 121:65–94.

Biederman J (1998): Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A life-span perspective. J Clin Psychiatry 59(suppl 7):4 –16. Bylsma FW, Pivik RT (1989): The effects of background

illumination and stimulant medication on smooth pursuit eye movements of hyperactive children. J Abnorm Psychol 17: 73–90.

Chen Y, Levy DL, Nakayama K, Matthysse S, Palafox G, Holzman PS (1999a): Dependence of impaired eye tracking on deficient velocity discrimination in schizophrenia. Arch

Gen Psychiatry 56:155–161.

Chen Y, Palafox GP, Nakayama K, Levy D, Mattthysse S, Holzman PS (1999b): Motion perception in schizophrenia.

Arch Gen Psychiatry 56:149 –154.

Clementz BA (1998): Psychophysiological measures of (dis)in-hibition as liability indicators for schizophrenia.

Psychophys-iology 35:648 – 668.

Clementz BA, Sweeney JA (1990): Is eye movement dysfunction a biological marker for schizophrenia: A methodological review. Psychol Bull 108:77–92.

Cohen J (1988): Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral

Sciences. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Diefendorf AR, Dodge R (1908): An experimental study of the ocular reaction on the insane from photographic records.

Brain 31:451– 489.

Farber RH, Clementz BA, Swerdlow NR (1997): Characteristics of open- and closed-loop smooth pursuit responses among obsessive-compulsive disorder, schizophrenia, and nonpsy-chiatric individuals. Psychophysiology 34:157–162. First MB, Spitzer RL, Gibbon M, Wingerson DK (1997):

Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders— Patient Edition. New York: New York State Psychiatric

Institute.

(7)

Friedman L, Abel LA, Jesberge JA, Malki A, Meltzer HY (1992): Saccadic intrusions into smooth pursuit in patients with schizophrenia or affective disorder and normal controls.

Biol Psychiatry 31:1110 –1118.

Gaymard B, Pierrot-Deseilligny C (1999): Neurology of sac-cades and smooth pursuit. Curr Opin Neurol 12:13–19. Holzman PS, Kringlen E, Matthysse S, Flanagan SD, Lipton RB,

Cramer G, et al (1988): A single dominant gene can account for eye tracking dysfunction and schizophrenia in offspring of discordant twins. Arch Gen Psychiatry 45:641– 647. Holzman PS, Solomon CM, Levin S, Waternaux CS (1984):

Pursuit eye movement dysfunctions in schizophrenia. Arch

Gen Psychiatry 41:136 –139.

Hutton JT, Nagel JA, Loewenson RB (1993): Variables affecting eye tracking performance. Electroencephalogr Clin

Neuro-physiol 56:414 – 419.

Jacobsen LK, Hong WL, Hommer DW, Hamgurger SD, Castel-lanos FX, Frazier JA, et al (1996): Smooth pursuit eye movements in childhood onset schizophrenia: Comparison with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder and normal con-trols. Biol Psychiatry 40:1144 –1154.

Kanayama R, Nakamura T, Sano R, Ohki M, Okuyama T, Kimura Y, Koike Y (1994): Effect of aging on mooth pursuit eye movement. Acta Otolaryngol Suppl (Stockh) 511:131– 134.

Kuechenmeister CA, Linton PH, Mueller TV, White HB (1977): Eye tracking in relation to age, sex, and illness. Arch Gen

Psychiatry 34:578 –579.

Larsby B, Thell J, Moller C, Odkvist L (1988): The effect of stimulus predictability and age on human tracking eye move-ments. Acta Otolaryngol (Stockh) 105:21–30.

Levy DL, Holzman PS, Matthysse S, Mendell NR (1993): Eye dysfunction and schizophrenia: A critical review. Schizophr

Res 19:461–536.

Lisberger SG, Pavelko TA (1989): Topographic and directional organization of visual motion inputs for initiation of horizon-tal and vertical smooth pursuit eye movements in monkeys.

J Neurophysiol 61:173–185.

Litman R, Hommer DW, Radant AD, Clem T, Pickar D (1994): Quantitative effects of typical and atypical neuroleptics on smooth pursuit eye tracking in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 12:107–120.

Olincy A, Ross RG, Young DA, Roath M, Freedman R (1998): Improvement in smooth pursuit eye movements after ciga-rette smoking in schizophrenic patients.

Neuropsychophar-macology 18:175–185.

Radant AD, Hommer DW (1992): A quantitative analysis of saccades and smooth pursuit during visual pursuit tracking: A comparison of schizophrenics with normals and substance abusing controls. Schizophr Res 6:225–235.

Rosenberg DR, Sweeney JA, Squires-Wheeler E, Keshavan MS, Cornblatt BA, Erlenmeyer-Kimling L (1997): Eye tracking dysfunction in offspring from the New York High Risk Project: Diagnostic specificity and the role of attention.

Psychiatry Res 66:121–130.

Ross DE, Buchanan RW, Medoff D, Lahti AC, Thaker GK (1998): Association between eye tracing disorder in schizo-phrenia and poor sensory integration. Am J Psychiatry 155: 1352–1357.

Ross RG, Harris JG, Cullum CM, Rilling LM, Radant AD, Adler LE, Freedman R (1997): Smooth pursuit eye movements in parents of probands with schizophrenia: An obligate carrier approach. Soc Neurosci Abstr 20:1262.

Ross RG, Harris JG, Olincy A, Radant AD, Adler LE, Freedman R (1998a): Familial transmission of two independent saccadic abnormalities in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 30:59 –70. Ross RG, Hommer DW, Breiger D, Varley C, Radant AD

(1994): Eye movement task related to frontal lobe functioning in children with attention deficit disorder. J Am Acad Child

Adolesc Psychiatry 33:869 – 874.

Ross RG, Hommer DW, Radant AD, Roath M, Freedman R (1996): Early expression of smooth pursuit eye movement abnormalities in children of schizophrenic parents. J Am Acad

Child Adolesc Psychiatry 35:941–949.

Ross RG, Olincy A, Harris JG, Radant A, Adler LE, Compagnon N, Freedman R (1999a): The effects of age on a smooth pursuit tracking task in adults with schizophrenia and nor-mals. Biol Psychiatry 46:383–391.

Ross RG, Olincy A, Harris JG, Radant AD, Adler LE, Freedman R (1998b): Anticipatory saccades during smooth pursuit eye movements and familial transmission of schizophrenia. Biol

Psychiatry 8:690 – 697.

Ross RG, Olincy A, Radant A (in press): What duration of post-saccadic slowing identifies anticipatory saccades during smooth pursuit eye movements? Psychophysiology.

Ross RG, Olincy A, Radant AD (1999b): Amplitude criteria and anticipatory saccades during smooth pursuit eye movements in schizophrenia. Psychophysiology 36:464 – 468.

Ross RG, Radant AD, Hommer DW (1993): A developmental study of smooth pursuit eye movements in normal children from 7 to 15 years of age. J Am Acad Child Adolesc

Psychiatry 32:783–791.

Roy-Byrne PP, Radant AD, Wingerson DK, Cowley DS (1995): Human oculomotor function: Reliability and diurnal varia-tion. Biol Psychiatry 38:92–97.

Shapira YA, Jones MH, Sherman SP (1980): Abnormal eye movements in hyperkinetic children with learning disability.

Neuropa¨diatrie 11:36 – 44.

Sharpe JA, Sylvester TO (1978): Effect of aging on horizontal smooth pursuit. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci 17:465– 468. Spooner JW, Sakala SM, Baloh RW (1980): Effect of aging on

eye tracking. Arch Neurol 37:575–576.

Strik WK, Dierks T, Boning J, Osterheider M, Caspari A, Korber J (1992): Disorders of smooth pursuit eye movements and auditory N100 in schizophrenic patients. Psychiatry Res 41:227–235.

Sweeney JA, Clementz BA, Haas GL, Escobar MD, Drake K, Frances AJ (1994): Eye tracking dysfunction in schizophre-nia: Characterization of component eye movement abnormal-ities, diagnostic specificity, and the role of attention. J

Abnorm Psychol 103:222–230.

Ward MF, Wender PH, Reimherr FW (1993): The Wender Utah Rating Scale: An aid in the retrospective diagnosis of child-hood attention deficit disorder. Am J Psychiatry 150:885– 890.

Whicker L, Abel LA, Dell’Osso LF (1985): Smooth pursuit eye movements in the parents of schizophrenics.

Gambar

Table 1. Demographics for Adults with Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD), Schizophrenia, and a NormalComparison Group
Table 2. Smooth Pursuit Performance in Adults with Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD), Schizophrenia, and aNormal Comparison Group
Figure 1. Smooth pursuit gain (eye velocity/target velocity) forschizophrenic, attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD),and normal adults during a 16.7°/sec tracking task.
Figure 2. Frequency of leading saccades (number/min) forschizophrenic, attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD),and normal adults during a 16.7°/sec tracking task.

Referensi

Dokumen terkait

Sekolah Menjalin kemitraan dalam rangka perlindungan dan pengelolaan lingkungan hidup dengan berbagai pihak (masyarakat, pemerintah, swasta, media, sekolah lain)... PENGELOLAAN

DILEMA MASYARAKAT SUMEDANG TERHADAP KEHADIRAN GERAKAN DI/TII TAHUN 1950-1962.. Universitas Pendidikan Indonesia | repository.upi.edu | perpustakaan.upi.edu

PROGRAM ADIWIYATA adalah program untuk mewujudkan sekolah yang peduli dan berbudaya lingkungan.. SEKOLAH ADIWIYATA : adalah sekolah peduli dan

mengalami peningkatan kandungan asam laktat seiring dengan peningkatan lama inkubasi, hingga lama inkubasi 10 hari, dan selanjutnya mengalami stagnasi sampai lama inkubasi

Daftar Peserta Seleksi Profile Assessment dan Wawancara Calon Hakim Ad Hoc Pengadilan Tindak Pidana Korupsi Tahap IV sebagai berikut

[r]

Perhitungan Statistik Pertambahan Ukuran Tinggi Pundak, Panjang Badan, Lingkar Dada dan Bobot Badan Harian Cempe Prasapih pada Kambing Lokal Berdasarkan

Buku/Dokumen Manual SPMI adalah dokumen berisi petunjuk teknis tentang cara, langkah, atau prosedur Penetapan, Pelaksanaan, Evaluasi pelaksanaan,