• Tidak ada hasil yang ditemukan

First Language Acquisition

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2023

Membagikan "First Language Acquisition"

Copied!
24
0
0

Teks penuh

(1)

Lecture 16:

Introduction to Linguistics – LANE 321 Lecturer: Haifa Alroqi

Chapter 14: First language acquisition

(2)

First Language Acquisition

The capacity to learn language is deeply ingrained in us as a species, just as the capacity to walk, to grasp objects, to recognize faces. We don‟t find any serious difference in children growing up in congested urban slums, in isolated mountain villages, or in privileged

suburban villas” Dan Slobin, The Human Language (1994)

(3)

First Language Acquisition

Every language is complex.

Before the age of 5, the child knows most of the complicated system of grammar.

Use the syntactic, phonological, morphological and semantic rules of the language.

Join sentences.

Ask questions.

Use appropriate pronouns.

Negate sentences.

Form relative clauses.

(4)

Issues in first language acquisition

How do children acquire such a complex system so quickly and effortlessly?

Does a child decide to consciously pursue certain skills? (e.g., walking)

Do babies make a conscious decision to start learning a language?

In spite of different backgrounds, different locations, and different upbringings, most children follow the very same milestones in acquiring language.

We correct children‟s errors sometimes. Does it help?

(5)

Basic requirements

1. A child requires interaction with other language-users in order to bring the general language capacity s/he has into operation.

Genie

Cultural transmission

2. The child must be physically capable.

Being able to speak

Being able to hear

Is hearing enough?

Interaction (The crucial requirement)

All these requirements are related.

(6)

The acquisition schedule

All normal children develop language at roughly the same time, along the same schedule.

Biological development – physical activities.

The biological schedule is very much related to the maturation of the infant‟s brain to cope with the linguistic input.

Children have the biological capacity to cope with certain aspects of linguistics

„input‟ at different stages during the early years of life.

Young children acquire the language by identifying the regularities in what is heard and applying those regularities in what they say.

(7)

Caretaker Speech (Motherese)

Caregiver/ Child directed speech

OK, Hammodi! Now Mammy will push choo choo!

A type of simplified speech adopted by someone who spends time with the child characterized by:

Frequent use of questions

Simplified lexicon

Phonological reduction

Higher pitch- extra loudness

Stressed intonation

Simple sentences

A lot of repetition

Baby-talk

simplified forms (tummy, )

alternative forms, with repeated simple sounds and syllables (nanna, nono, humm, dada, haw-haw, cocococo, choo-choo)

(8)

Caretaker speech (Motherese)

Assign interactive roles to young children even before they become speaking participants.

MOTHER: Look!

CHILD: (touches picture)

MOTHER: what are those?

CHILD: (vocalizes a babble string and smiles)

MOTHER: yes, there are rabbits

CHILD: (vocalizes, smiles looks up at mother)

MOTHER: (laughs) yes, rabbit

CHILD: (vocalizes, smiles)

MOTHER: Yes. (laughs)

(9)

L1 acquisition

Stage Typical age Description

Cooing 3-5 months Speech-like sounds

Babbling 6-11 months Repetitive CV patterns

One-word stage 12-18 months Single open-class words or word stems

Two-word stage 18-20 months "mini-sentences" with simple semantic relations

Telegraphic speech 24-30 months sentence structures of lexical words no functional or grammatical morphemes

Later multiword stage 30+ months Grammatical or functional structures emerge

(10)

Cooing

The first few months: the child becomes capable of producing

sequences of vowel-like sounds- high vowels [i] and [u].

4 months:

the ability to bring the back of the tongue into regular contact with the back of the palate – producing sounds similar to velar consonants [k] & [g] (cooing & gooing)

5 months:

distinguish between the vowels [a] and [i] and the

syllables [ba] and [ga] -perception skills.

(11)

Babbling

6-8 months: sitting up & producing a number of different vowels, consonants, and combinations like ba-ba-ba and ga-ga-ga (babbling)

9-10 months:

Variation in the combinations (ba-ba-da-da)

Nasal sounds become common

Certain syllable sequences also appear (ma-ma-ma & da-da-da)

10-11months:

use vocalization to express emotions & emphasis.

More complex syllable combination (ma-da-ga-ba)

A lot of sound play & attempted imitations.

Note:

Even deaf children babble.

There is substantial variation among children in terms of the age at which particular features of linguistic development occur.

(12)

The one-word stage

12-18 months.

recognizable single-unit utterances.

single terms are uttered for everyday objects „milk‟, „cookie‟, „cat‟,

„cup‟, and „spoon‟ [pun].

Holoprastic (wasa = what's that)

Produce utterances such as „Sara bed‟ but not yet capable of producing a more complex phrase.

(13)

The two-word stage

18-20 months: vocabulary moves beyond 50 words

By 2 years old, children produce utterances „baby chair‟, „mommy eat‟

Interpretation depends on context.

Baby chair might mean:

This is baby‟s chair

Put baby in chair

Baby is in the chair

Adults behave as if communication is taking place.

The child not only produces speech, but receives feedback confirming that the utterance worked as a contribution to the interaction.

By this age, whether the child is producing 200 or 300 words, he or she will be capable of understanding 5 times as many.

(14)

Telegraphic speech

2-2½ years:

The child produces „multiple-word‟ speech.

The child has already developed sentence-building capacity & can get the word order correct ( „cat drink milk‟, „daddy go bye-bye’)

A number of grammatical inflections begin to appear.

Simple prepositions (in, on) are also used

Vocabulary is expanding rapidly.

3 years:

Vocabulary has grown more.

Better pronunciation

(15)

The acquisition process

For most children, no one provides any instruction on how to speak the language.

Children actively construct, from what is said to them, possible ways of using the language.

trying out constructions and testing whether they work or not.

So, children acquire language not only by imitating.

They produce expressions adults never produced.

(e.g. p. 154)

(16)

The acquisition process

It is also unlikely that adult‟s „correction‟ are very effective of how the child speaks. (p.149)

CHILD: my teacher holded the baby rabbits and we patted them MOTHER: did you say your teacher held the baby rabbits?

CHILD: yes.

MOTHER: What did you say she did?

CHILD she holded the baby rabbit and we patted them.

MOTHER: Did you say she held them tightly?

CHILD: no, she holded them loosely

(17)

The acquisition process

One important factor in the development of the child's linguistic repertoire is the actual use of sound &word combinations.

In interactions with others

In word play. (p. 155)

(18)

Developing morphology

By 2-2½ years: use of some inflectional morphemes to indicate the grammatical function of nouns and verbs.

The 1st inflection to appear is -ing (cat sitting, mommy reading book)

after it comes the –s for regular plural. (boys, cats)

The acquisition of the plural marker is often accompanied by a process of overgeneralization.

The child overgeneralizes and applies –s to words like „foots‟ „mans‟. (p.

155)

(19)

Developing morphology

Not long after, the use of possessive „s‟ appears (mommy’s bag)

At about the same time, forms of verb to be appear (is, are, was)

At about the same time of the appearance of was, irregular verbs (went, came) appear.

Then, The -ed for past tense appears.

Once it appears, the irregular verb forms disappear for a while replaced by overgeneralized versions (goed, comed)

Finally, the -s marker for 3rd person singular present tense verbs appears with full verbs first (comes, looks) then with auxiliaries (does, has)

(20)

Developing syntax

A child was asked to say the owl who eats candy runs fast and she said the owl eat candy and he run fast.

It is clear that the child understands what the adult is saying. She just has her own way of expressing it.

Stages of

Forming questions

Forming negatives

(21)

Forming questions

1st stage:

Two procedures:

Add a Wh-form to the beginning of the expression (e.g. Where horse go?)

Utter the expression with a rise in intonation toward the end. (e.g. Sit chair?)

2nd stage:

More complex expressions can be formed.

The rising intonation strategy continues to be used.

More Wh-forms.

e.g. What book name?/Why you smiling?/You want eat?/See my doggie?

3rd stage:

Inversion of subject and verb

e.g. will you help me?/ What did I do?/ Can I go?

(22)

Forming negative

Stage 1:

Putting not or no at the beginning

e.g. not teddy bear, no sit here, no go home

Stage 2:

Don’t and can’t appear.

no and not before verbs rather than at the beginning of the sentence.

e.g., I don’t want it, you can’t dance, he no bite you. Mommy no go

Stage 3:

didn’t and won’t appear.

The typical stage 1 forms disappear.

Isn’t is very late to be acquired (some stage 2 forms continue to be used for quite a long time)

e.g. I didn’t caught it, she won’t let go, he not taking it, This not ice cream

(23)

Developing Semantics

During the two-word stage children use their limited vocabulary to refer to a large number of unrelated objects.

Overextension: overextend the meaning of a word on the basis of similarities of shape, sound, and size.

e.g. use ball to refer to (all kinds of round objects) an apple, and egg, a grape, the moon, a doorknob, and a ball.

This is followed by a gradual process of narrowing down the application of each term as more words are learned.

Hyponymy – children will almost use the middle-level term in a hyponymous set as animal – dog – poodle

Antonymy - Antonymous relations are acquired late (after the age of 5) (e.g.

p. 159)

(24)

Thank you

Referensi

Dokumen terkait

NATIONAL INSTITUTE OF TECHNOLOGY DEPARTMENT OF HUMNITIES AND SOCIAL SCIENCES TIRUCHIRAPPALLI – 620015 1 of 4 Tender Notification Number: NITT/F.No:001/REV.EXP.31/2017-18/HUM