• Tidak ada hasil yang ditemukan

Automatic eye fixations identification b

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2017

Membagikan "Automatic eye fixations identification b"

Copied!
21
0
0

Teks penuh

(1)

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/220644280

Automatic eye fixations identification based on

analysis of variance and covariance

Article in Pattern Recognition Letters · October 2011

DOI: 10.1016/j.patrec.2011.06.012 · Source: DBLP

CITATIONS

6

READS

100

6 authors, including:

Giacomo Veneri

General Electric

18PUBLICATIONS 59CITATIONS SEE PROFILE

Pietro Piu

Università degli Studi di Siena

30PUBLICATIONS 92CITATIONS SEE PROFILE

Francesca Rosini

Università degli Studi di Siena

35PUBLICATIONS 127CITATIONS SEE PROFILE

Pamela Federighi

Università degli Studi di Siena

27PUBLICATIONS 92CITATIONS SEE PROFILE

(2)

Automatic Eye Fixations Identification based on

Analysis of Variance and Covariance

Giacomo Veneria,b,c,

, Pietro Piub

, Francesca Rosinib

, Pamela Federighia,b , Antonio Federicob, Alessandra Rufaa,b,

a

Eye tracking & Vision Applications Lab - University of Siena

b

Department of Neurological Neurosurgical and Behavioral Science - University of Siena

c

Etruria Innovazione Spa

Abstract

Eye movement is the simplest and repetitive movement that enables humans to interact with the environment. The common daily activities, such as reading a book or watching television, involve this natural activity, which consists of rapidly shifting our gaze from one region to another. In clinical application, the identification of the main components of eye movement dur-ing visual exploration, such as fixations and saccades, is the objective of the analysis of eye movements: however, in patients affected by motor control disorder the identification of fixation is not banal. This work presents a new fixation identification algorithm based on the analysis of variance and co-variance: the main idea was to use bivariate statistical analysis to compare variance over x and y to identify fixation. We describe the new algorithm, and we compare it with the common fixations algorithm based on dispersion. To demonstrate the performance of our approach, we tested the algorithm in a group of healthy subjects and patients affected by motor control disorder.

Keywords: Eye Tracking, Fixation identification, Analysis of Variance, Motor Control Disorder

Department of Neurological Neurosurgical and Behavioral Science - University of Siena, Viale Bracci 2, 53100 Siena, Phone +39-0577-233136, Fax +39-0577-40327

(3)

1. Introduction 1

Eye movements are an essential part of human vision as they drive the 2

fovea and, consequently, visual attention toward a region of interest in the 3

space. This enables visual system to process an image or its details with a 4

high resolution power (Privitera and Stark, 2000). 5

The study of eye movements is an up-and-coming tool to study neurolog-6

ical disorders in clinical applications. Voluntary eye movements (saccades, 7

smooth pursuit) are controlled by several structures in the central nervous 8

system, which may enable easier distinction between peripheral and central 9

lesions (Juhola et al., 2007). Brain’s structures of the paramedian pontine 10

reticular formation and the vestibulo-cerebellum are involved in the coor-11

dination of eye movements and in vestibular responses (Leigh R.J., 2006). 12

Some other neurological diseases, such as cerebellar ataxia, have an influence 13

on saccade velocity, accuracy or latency. 14

Therefore, a correct analysis of eye movements can lead to distinguish pa-15

tients from healthy subjects. 16

The fixations and saccades are the main features of eye movements; fixations 17

are samples of points around a centre point (centroid) with long duration 18

(>> 50ms); these eye fixations are intercalated by rapid eye jumps

(sac-19

cade), which can be defined as rapid eye movement with velocities that may 20

be higher than 500 deg/sec and duration about 20-40 ms (Ramat et al., 21

2007); Fig. 1 shows a small portion of gaze sample during visual exploration 22

on a psychological task: it’s easy to identify three clusters of data points 23

(fixations) and two saccades. 24

25

From a psychological point of view, the fixation is defined as the act 26

of maintaining the visual gaze on a single location in order to make our 27

environment visible (see Martinez-Conde et al. (2004) for a review of the role 28

of fixations). From a technical point of view, fixation should be identified by 29

a cluster of points around a centroid with a minimum duration; Irwin et al. 30

(1990) found the theoretical minimum duration for a single fixation to be 31

150 ms, whereas Manor and Gordon (2003) argued that 100 ms can also be 32

justified. Rayner (1998) indicated that the mean duration of a single fixation 33

may depend on the nature of the task (225 ms on reading, 275 ms on visual 34

search, 400 ms hand-eye coordination). 35

During fixation, the eye does not remain completely stable, but is affected by 36

(4)

0 200 400 600 800 1000 1200 0

100 200 300 400 500 600 700 800

x(t)

y(t)

Fixation 2 Saccade

Fixation 3

Fixation 1

Saccade

(a) x(t) y(t)

0 500 1000

Position (px)

1600 1800 2000 2200 2400 2600 2800 3000 3200 3400 10−5

100

p−Value ,

ρw

(log−scale)

Time (ms)

x y

F−Test p−value

ρw

Fixation

1

Fixation

2

Fixation

3

(b) x(t) and y(t), distance covariance and p-value (bottom graph).

Figure 1: A small portion of gaze of subject GV. For each sample (x(τ), y(τ)) at timeτ

(5)

making it difficult to easily identify it by an algorithm. 38

1.1. Related works 39

In order to implement an efficient algorithm able to identify automat-40

ically fixations, the efforts have been concentrated on three parameters : 41

fixations duration, dispersion and velocity. Generally speaking, the most 42

common algorithms are based on clustering analysis (Urruty et al., 2007) or 43

dispersion thresholding: Distance Dispersion Algorithm, Centroid-Distance 44

Method (Anliker, 1976), Position-Variance Method and Salvucci I-DT Algo-45

rithm (Salvucci and Goldberg, 2000). 46

In Distance Dispersion Algorithm each point in that fixation must be no 47

further than some threshold dmax from every other point. Position-Variance

48

requires that M of N points have a standard deviation of distance from the 49

centroid not exceeding σmax.Centroid-Distance requires that M of N points

50

be no further than some threshold from the centroid of the N points. Salvucci 51

I-DT requires that the maximal horizontal distance and the maximal vertical 52

distance is less than threshold. 53

Shic et al. (2008) accomplished a comparison of this algorithms: they found 54

that choices in analysis can lead to very different interpretations of the same 55

eye-tracking data. Blignaut (2009) found that the correct setting of disper-56

sion threshold on I-DT is of utmost importance, especially if the subjects are 57

not homogeneous. 58

1.2. Objective 59

We aimed to develop a robust algorithm able to identify fixations in clini-60

cal cases. The dispersion algorithm of I-DT Algorithm is accounted to be the 61

most robust method (Shic et al., 2008), we implemented the new algorithm 62

(Fixation Dispersion algorithm based on Covariance - C-DT) based on the 63

similar principle, but we evaluated the bivariate variance around the cen-64

troid and the covariance to classify the data point. In Veneri et al. (2010b) 65

we used the F-test of equal variance to identify fixations: the key principle 66

of the proposed technique was based on supposing the variance along the 67

x was not significantly different from that along the y during fixations: the 68

algorithm was able to identify more fixations than I-DT, but in clinical cases, 69

where eye movements should be affected by brain diseases, the assumptions 70

such as the normality of distribution or the number of data points should af-71

fect the efficacy of the algorithm. For these reasons we used a mixed method 72

(6)

The first section of the paper describes the implemented algorithm, then a 74

comparison between the C-DT and the I-DT Algorithm applied to simulated 75

(artificial) fixations is proposed; the final section is devoted to a case study 76

in normal subjects and patients. 77

2. Material and methods 78

2.1. Simulated fixations 79

In order to assess the proposed algorithm C-DT, we simulated 3500 fixa-80

tions plus a small portion of two saccades at the endpoint of fixation. Fixation 81

was defined as two dimensional sample vector data points x(t), y(t) around 82

the fixation’s centroidxc(t), yc(t), distributed with random Gaussian variance

83

Beers model (van Beers, 2008) with maximum velocity equal to 600 deg/s

85

(Ramat et al., 2007). 86

The duration of fixation ranged from 50 to 550ms. The variance over the x 87

and y ranged from 3 px2 to 50 px2, which corresponded to a max dispersion

88

ranging from 10pxto 300px. 89

2.2. Subjects’ fixations 90

Fourteen healthy subjects and six patients with (diagnosed and well 91

known) eye motor control disorder were enrolled aged 25-45. Subjects were 92

seated at viewing distance of 78cm from a 32” color monitor (51cm×31cm). 93

Eye position was recorded using ASL 6000 system, which consists of a remote-94

mounted camera sampling pupil location at 240Hz. A 9-point calibration and 95

3-point validation procedure was repeated several times to ensure all record-96

ings had a mean spatial error of less than 0.3degree. Data was controlled by 97

a Pentium4 dual core 3GHz computer, which acquires signals by fast UART 98

serial port. Head movement was restricted using a chin rest and bite. Sub-99

ject was asked to fixate a centered red dot on the center; after 500ms the 100

centered red dot disappeared and subject could explore the scene. The dis-101

played scenes were randomly choosen in a collection of real images (Privitera 102

and Stark, 2000) or pop-up images. The fixations were manually identified 103

by an expert operator. 104

(7)

3. Theory 106

In human visual search the source of variability should be due to the same 107

system (Beers, 2007; Veneri et al., 2011); the key principle of the proposed 108

technique is based on supposing x and y independent with the same variance 109

during a fixation. 110

The method is based on this assumption, and evaluates the hypothesis by 111

distance covariance coefficient and a statistical method such as the F-test 112

for equal variance; the F-test is used to verify that two populations (with 113

normal distribution) have the same mean or variance (this is the so called 114

”null hypothesis” H0 to be tested against the alternative complementary 115

hypothesis H1 that the two populations have heterogeneous variances) and 116

is a standard statistical procedure. In an informal way the F test calculates 117

the F distribution 118

between group variability

within group variability =

variability over x or y

variability between x y (1)

and, then, evaluates the probability to get a result of the test less than 119

the one actually observed when the null hypothesis H0 is true (in the C-DT 120

algorithm there is no difference between the two variances): this probability is 121

often indicated p-value or simply p. We must note that F-test was diagnosed 122

as being extremely sensitive to non-normality (Markowski, 1990). 123

We assumed that x and y on fixations come from a normal distribution with 124

same variance: then the highest relative p-values that did not accept the null 125

hypothesis (greater than 5%) were classified as the centroid of the fixation. 126

The assumption of equal variance and the normality of the distribution, may 127

be eligible for normal subjects, but in the case of patients affected by motor 128

control disorders, the fixation is affected by nystagmus, drifts, square jerks 129

(Leigh R.J., 2006) and the method should fail. Covariance is a measure of 130

how much the deviations of two or more variables or processes match and 131

should be a good candidate to replace or integrate the F-test. Covariance is 132

where x is the mean of vector x. If x and y are independent, then their 134

(8)

reasons the correlation coefficient (Sz´ekely et al., 2007) (or a bivariate non-136

parametric test (Feuerverger, 1993)) is more suitable. Correlation coefficient 137

is defined as 138

ρ(x, y) = cov(x, y)

σx·σy

(3)

whereσx and σy are the standard deviations of x and y respectively and

139

the product provides the normalisation factor to hold the Cauchy-Schwarz 140

inequality 0≤ρ(x, y)≤1. 141

Formally, the classification problem should be defined by: given a data point 142

x(t), y(t) at time t 1

143

x(t), y(t)→F if |ρ(x(τ), y(τ))| ≤k ∀τ ∈(t−w/2,t+w/2) (4)

where (t−w/2, t+w/2) is a small time window, Fis the set of fixations 144

andkis a constant (threshold)∈(0,1). From an intuitive point of view, when 145

x and y are randomly distributed, they are independent and ρ(x, y)≈0. 146

4. Calculation 147

The Eq. 4 is theoretically correct, but it was very sensitive to microsac-148

cade, small changes and depended strictly from k. We used a windowed 149

Pearson’s Correlation Coefficient defined as: 150

ρw(x, y,t) =

|cov(x(τ), y(τ))| σxσy

∀τ ∈(t−w/2,t+w/2) (5)

wherew was set to the minimum “physiological wave” to take in consid-151

eration, which is, in our case, the saccade: w= 20−25ms≈6sampleandσx

152

is the variance ofx. The Eq. 5 does not hold the Cauchy-Schwarz inequality, 153

but keeps the independent condition for ρw = 0.

154

To develop a more robust technique we integrated the proposed method with 155

an empirical mixed method based on covariance distance and F-test: 156

1

(9)

ξw(x, y,t) =

determining the degree of freedom and evaluating the F function = σ2 x/σ

2 y

159

with n-1 and n-1 degree of freedom and, finally, finding the p-value on the 160

F-distribution table. β is a very small empirical threshold ∼= 0.01, and α is 161

the standard confidence interval of p-value: α1 = 0.05 andα2 = 0.01. Finally

162

we applied Eq. 4 for 163

k =ξw(x, y,t) ∀(x(t), y(t)) when pw(x, y,t)> α1 (7)

From an intuitive point of view, k is evaluated as mean of Eq. 6 only for 164

consideration anything else than dispersion of fixations, avoiding any consid-170

eration on velocity or duration of fixation. 171

Starting from the centroid, the algorithm identifies the entire fixation 175

4.1. Algorithm description 179

In summary: (x,y) where F-test for equal variance accepts the H0 is in 180

(10)

are classified according to covariance and threshold k estimated through F-182

test for equal variance. The pseudo code A. 1 describes the algorithm: 183

1. the covariance distance and equal variance are evaluated on a small 184

time window (Eq. 6) 185

2. threshold constant k is estimated through F-test for equal variance 186

(Eq. 7) 187

3. the points (x,y) having the same variance or small covariance (≤k) are 188

assigned to fixations set (Eq. 4) 189

4. the fixations are group of contiguous points (Eq. 8) 190

5. the fixation is extended until variances product remains the same (Eq. 9) 191

5. Results 192

5.1. Simulated fixations 193

Fig. 2 and Fig. 3 show C-DT compared to I-DT. I-DT failed when dis-194

persion was greater than the maximum dispersion admitted by algorithm. 195

We cannot consider this being an error, but it’s the main characteristic of 196

the I-DT algorithm; similar result was found by Shic et al. (2008). The C-197

DT does not consider maximum dispersion, but only the covariance between 198

variance over x and y. 199

Generally speaking the C-DT identified simulated fixations with a preci-200

sion of 87% and I-DT of 42%. The performance of I-DT, however, strictly 201

depends on the threshold dispersion set; when we chose a large threshold 202

the I-DT identified more fixations, but in a real case should be affected by 203

incorrect clasification of saccade. 204

C-DT failed when it identified one fixation as two fixations (over segmenta-205

tion problem). 206

5.2. Case study 207

The fixations of fourteen healthy subjects and six patients with cerebellar 208

disorders were analyzed: C-DT identified 88,11% of fixations. I-DT was set 209

to max dispersion threshold (∼= 110px) and identified 63,86% of fixations. 210

The algorithms I-DT and C-DT were applied to raw data; when we applied 211

a low-pass filter (Butter-worth filter, 3rd order, fc = 2.1Hz) the

perfor-212

mances improved to∼93%, but it depends from the type of filter and filter’s 213

(11)

0

20

40 0

20

40 0

0.5 1

σx2 (px2) I−DT

σy2 (px2)

% correct identification

0

20

40 0

20

40 0

0.5 1

σx2 (px2) C−DT

σy2 (px2)

% correct identification

Figure 2: Percentage of correctly identified fixations by I-DT and C-DT varying variance of x axis, y axis and duration of fixation. Performance of I-DT and C-DT are similar for small values of σx and σy; when dispersion of x and/or y increased (max dispersion varied 150pxto 300px) I-DT was not able to identify fixation as expected, on the contrary, performance of C-DT remained stable. Performance of C-DT appeared to decrease when

(12)

0 100 200 300 400 500 600 0.1

0.2 0.3 0.4 0.5 0.6 0.7 0.8 0.9

% correct identification

duration

I−DT C−DT

(13)

Healthy subjects AR EU FC GV TS BO CH

By human operator 31 26 52 43 46 40 27

C-DT 28 23 43 38 34 35 29

rejected fixations by F-test 52% 60% 41% 67% 35% 60% 57%

I-DT 24 15 21 56 40 27 19

Healthy subjects EPE FF GR PP PT ANR SL

By human operator 30 73 28 70 19 55 40

C-DT 26 71 22 59 18 53 34

rejected fixations by F-test 57% 40% 46% 28% 86% 28% 48%

I-DT 23 56 23 57 35 61 16

Patients GG LB SDN XP XV CN GF

By human operator 36 56 32 60 59 66 51

C-DT 40 48 24 52 55 65 48

rejected fixations by F-test 61% 51% 66% 61% 50% 60% 80%

I-DT 31 30 11 49 42 36 16

(14)

Figure 4 shows a small portion of gaze of subject GV and EU. C-DT 215

identified ∼= 90% of fixations. 216

217

Figure 5 shows a small portion of an exploration made by subject FC and 218

AR: C-DT identified the fixations (thick line) also on high variance cases. 219

Figure 6 shows the exploration made by patients: C-DT was insensitive 220

to drifts, but failed for artifacts such as spikes. 221

An empirical analysis of lost fixations was performed; I-DT failed to iden-222

tify correctly large fixations or very short fixations (≤60ms). The short fixa-223

tions, however, are classified by some authors saccade interruptions (Findlay 224

et al., 2001). C-DT failed on the identification a single fixation as two or more 225

fixations (over segmentation problem) or on grouping two fixations separated 226

by a very small saccade. In any case, a close comparison between I-DT and 227

C-DT does not make sense because they work with different techniques and 228

very often it is not trivial to define a “fixation”. 229

6. Conclusion 230

In the last decade a large effort has been made to identify fixations (An-231

liker, 1976; Salvucci and Goldberg, 2000; Urruty et al., 2007; Blignaut, 2009), 232

however it is not yet easy to provide a formal mathematical definition of fix-233

ation: some authors have demonstrated that fixation’s parameters depend 234

strictly by the type of task (Rayner, 1998; Irwin et al., 1990; Manor and Gor-235

don, 2003; Shic et al., 2008). We suggested a formal definition of fixations 236

based on analysis of variance between x axis and y axis; the implemented 237

algorithm is based on the dispersion algorithm I-DT developed by Salvucci 238

and Goldberg (2000) and integrates it with a statistical test (F-test) and 239

covariance. The main advantage of the proposed technique is to provide a 240

new definition of fixation which does not require the setting of any critical 241

parameter or threshold, and provides a probability value of correctness. 242

Further work will be addressed to improve the extension mechanism described 243

in the algorithm 1 by a robust method such as the correntropy. Then, to avoid 244

the normality assumption, the F-test should be replaced by a non paramet-245

ric test, but it will require more points. Then, due to the high sensitivity of 246

F-test, we will evaluate the opportunity to integrate it on a real-time appli-247

cation such as gaze-contingent (Veneri et al., 2010a). 248

Finally we will investigate towards improving the system of recognition of 249

(15)

0 512

Position (px)

x y

500 1000 1500 2000 2500 3000

0 0.5 1

Time (ms) ξ w

ξw

k = 0.015822

(a) Subject GV

0 512

Position (px)

x y

4.2 4.25 4.3 4.35 4.4 4.45

x 104 0

0.5 1

Time (ms) ξ w

ξw

k = 0.002827

(16)

0 512

Position (px)

x y

4.05 4.1 4.15 4.2 4.25

x 104 0

0.5 1

Time (ms) ξ w

ξw

k = 0.002511

(a) Subject FC

0 512

Position (px)

x y

4.2 4.25 4.3 4.35 4.4 4.45 4.5 4.55 x 104 0

0.5 1

Time (ms) ξ w

ξ

w

k = 0.025633

σ = 6.82

(17)

0 512

Position (px)

x y

1500 2000 2500 3000 3500 4000

0 0.5 1

Time (ms) ξ w

ξw

k = 0.008190 drift

(a) Patient CN

0 512

Position (px)

x y

3.95 4 4.05 4.1 4.15

x 104 0

0.5 1

Time (ms) ξ w

ξ

w

k = 0.037346 drift

(18)

patients with severe ocular motor disorders such as nystagmus or tremor 251

(Federighi et al., 2011). In this kind of patients the source of variability 252

of fixations should be different between x and y and the algorithm should 253

fail; the difference, however, depends strictly on the subject and the ratio of 254

variability between x and y should be calculated a priori. 255

Acknowledgment 256

The authors would like to thank the editor and reviewers for improvements

257

and suggestions and Tuscany Region and Grado Zero Espace (Dr. Matteo Piccini)

258

for funding support (SISSI Project) on the current research.

259

Materials 260

Developed MATLAB algorithm should be downloaded at [THE JOURNAL

261

URL].

262

Appendix A. Algorithm 263

References 264

Anliker, L., 1976. Eye movements: On-line measurement, analysis, and control.

265

Eye movements and psychological processes , 185–199.

266

Beers, R.J., 2007. The sources of variability in saccadic eye movements. Journal

267

of Neuroscience 27, 8757–8770.

268

van Beers, R.J., 2008. Saccadic eye movements minimize the consequences of

269

motor noise. PLoS One 3, e2070.

270

Blignaut, P., 2009. Fixation identification: the optimum threshold for a dispersion

271

algorithm. Atten Percept Psychophys 71, 881–895.

272

Federighi, P., Cevenini, G., Dotti, M.T., Rosini, F., Pretegiani, E., Federico, A.,

273

Rufa, A., 2011. Differences in saccade dynamics between spinocerebellar ataxia

274

2 and late-onset cerebellar ataxias. Brain 134, 879–891.

275

Feuerverger, A., 1993. A consistent test for bivariate dependence. International

276

Statistical Review / Revue Internationale de Statistique 61, pp. 419–433.

(19)

Algorithm 1 Operator ”a:b” means ”all point between a and b”. Operator ”cov”, ”var” or ”ftest” are the functions which evaluates covariance, variance of a vector and ftest of two vectors. Operator ”relativemax” is a function which calculates the relative maximum of contiguous points.

1: {Calculate variance} 2: σx←var(x)

3: σy←var(y)

4: for all x(t), y(t) do

5: xˇ←x(t−w/2) :x(t+w/2) 6: yˇ←y(t−w/2) :y(t+w/2)

7: F(t)←f test(ˇx,yˇ){F-test on datax(t), y(t)t∈(−w/2, w/2)}

8: R(t)← |cov(ˇx,yˇ)|/(σxσy) 9: t←t+ ∆t{Move to next point} 10: if F(t)0.05 ANDR(t)0.01then

11: R(t)←0

12: end if

13: if F(t)0.01 ANDR(t)0.01then

14: R(t)←1

15: end if

16: end for

17: {Evaluate threshold}

18: χ←mean(R(τ))∀τ:F(τ)≥0.05 19: {Identify fixations centroid} 20: R∗Rχ

21: {Look for centroids}

22: xi, yi←relativemax(x(t), y(t), R∗)

23: {Extend fixation}

24: for all xi, yi do

25: xˇ←x(t−w/2) :x(t+w/2) 26: yˇ←y(t−w/2) :y(t+w/2)

27: {Calculate product of variances of x and y on data (xt, yt)t∈(−W, W)} 28: s←var(ˇx)·var(ˇy)

29: ss←s

30: t0←t−w/2

31: t1←t+w/2

32: while sssAND CONTINUEdo

33: xx←x(t0−∆t) :x(t1)

34: yy←y(t0−∆t) :y(t1)

35: ss←var(xx)·var(yy) 36: if sssthen

37: t0←t0−∆t

38: CONTINUE

39: end if

40: xx←x(t0) :x(t1+ ∆t)

41: yy←y(t0) :y(t1+ ∆t)

42: ss←var(xx)·var(yy) 43: if sssthen

44: t1←t1+ ∆t

45: CONTINUE

46: end if

47: end while

(20)

Findlay, J.M., Brown, V., Gilchrist, I.D., 2001. Saccade target selection in visual

278

search: the effect of information from the previous fixation. Vision Res 41,

279

87–95.

280

Hogg, R.V., Ledolter, J., 1987. Engineering Statistics. MacMillan.

281

Irwin, D.E., Zacks, J.L., Brown, J.S., 1990. Visual memory and the perception of

282

a stable visual environment. Percept Psychophys 47, 35–46.

283

Juhola, M., Aalto, H., Hirvonen, T., 2007. Using results of eye movement signal

284

analysis in the neural network recognition of otoneurological patients. Comput.

285

Methods Prog. Biomed. 86, 216–226.

286

Leigh R.J., Z.D., 2006. The neurology of eye movements (Book/DVD). Fourth

287

edition. New York: Oxford University Press.

288

Manor, B.R., Gordon, E., 2003. Defining the temporal threshold for ocular fixation

289

in free-viewing visuocognitive tasks. J Neurosci Methods 128, 85–93.

290

Markowski, Carol A; Markowski, E.P., 1990. Conditions for the effectiveness of a

291

preliminary test of variance. The American Statistician 44, 322326.

292

Martinez-Conde, S., Macknik, S.L., Hubel, D.H., 2004. The role of fixational eye

293

movements in visual perception. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 5, 229–240.

294

Privitera, C., Stark, L., 2000. Algorithms for defining visual regions-of-interest:

295

comparison with eye fixations. Pattern Analysis and Machine Intelligence, IEEE

296

Transactions on 22, 970–982.

297

Ramat, S., Leigh, R.J., Zee, D.S., Optican, L.M., 2007. What clinical disorders

298

tell us about the neural control of saccadic eye movements. Brain 130, 10–35.

299

Rayner, K., 1998. Eye movements in reading and information processing: 20 years

300

of research. Psychol Bull 124, 372–422.

301

Salvucci, D.D., Goldberg, J.H., 2000. Identifying fixations and saccades in

eye-302

tracking protocols, in: ETRA ’00: Proceedings of the 2000 symposium on Eye

303

tracking research & applications, ACM, New York, NY, USA. pp. 71–78.

304

Shic, F., Scassellati, B., Chawarska, K., 2008. The incomplete fixation measure,

305

in: ETRA ’08: Proceedings of the 2008 symposium on Eye tracking research

306

and applications, ACM, New York, NY, USA. pp. 111–114.

(21)

Sz´ekely, G.J., Bakirov, N.K., Rizzo, L.M., 2007. Measuring and testing

indepen-308

dence by correlation of distances. The Annals of Statistics 35, 2769–2794.

309

Urruty, T., Lew, S., Djeraba, C., Simovici, D.A., 2007. Detecting eye fixations by

310

projection clustering, in: Proc. 14th International Conference on Image Analysis

311

and Processing Workshops ICIAPW 2007, pp. 45–50.

312

Veneri, G., Federighi, P., Rosini, F., Federico, A., Rufa, A., 2010a. Influences of

313

data filtering on human-computer interaction by gaze-contingent display and

314

eye-tracking applications. Computers in Human Behavior 26, 1555 – 1563.

315

Veneri, G., Federighi, P., Rosini, F., Federico, A., Rufa, A., 2011. Spike removal

316

through multiscale wavelet and entropy analysis of ocular motor noise: A case

317

study in patients with cerebellar disease. Journal of Neuroscience Methods 196,

318

318–326.

319

Veneri, G., Piu, P., Federighi, P., Rosini, F., Federico, A., Rufa, A., 2010b. Eye

320

fixations identification based on statistical analysis - case study, in: Cognitive

321

Information Processing (CIP), 2010 2nd International Workshop on, IEEE. pp.

322

446 –451.

Gambar

Figure 1: A small portion of gaze of subject GV. For each sample (x(τ), y(τ)) at time τon a time window (t − w/2, t + w/2), we evaluated variance on x versus variance on y bybivariate F-test
Figure 2: Percentage of correctly identified fixations by I-DT and C-DT varying varianceof x axis, y axis and duration of fixation
Figure 3: Percentage of correct fixation identified by I-DT and C-DT changing the fixationduration.
Table 1: Fixations identified by algorithms C-TD and I-DT on fourteen healthy subjects
+4

Referensi

Dokumen terkait

Peningkatan serapan nitrogen (N) tanaman yang tercermin oleh meningkatnya konsentrasi nitrogen (N) dalam jaringan tanaman dan bobot kering tanaman dapat dipastikan

baku yang ekonomis yang dilakukan pada Industri Cita Rasaku pada bulan Februari sampai Mei 2014 dengan menggunakan metode EOQ (Economic Order Quantity)

Kepala Satuan Kerja Pekerjaan Pembangunan Prasarana dan Sarana Pemerintahan Daerah;. PPK Pekerjaan Pembangunan Prasarana dan Sarana Pemerintahan Daerah;

****) Jika ada, diisi dengan keterangan mutasi kepegawaian yang mengakibatkan pemberhentian / pembatalan tunjangan profesi / tunjangan kehormatan ( misalnya : meninggal TMT 15-2-

Pelaporan utam a m elalui online Sistem M onitoring TEP yang dikelola U nit Kerja Presiden Bldang Pengaw asan dan Pengendalian Pem bangunan, saat ini baru m elayani Kem

[r]

Dengan demikian, jika hipotesis berbasis perjanjian memberikan interpretasi yang valid dari hasil dasar makalah ini, maka manajer perusahaan memiliki Jiexibility akuntansi dan

Berdasarkan alat analisis yang digunakan maka peneliti telah memperoleh hasil bahwa variabel disiplin dan kepuasan kerja secara parsial berpengaruhg