• Tidak ada hasil yang ditemukan

Directory UMM :Data Elmu:jurnal:B:Biological Psichatry:Vol48.Issue4.2000:

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2017

Membagikan "Directory UMM :Data Elmu:jurnal:B:Biological Psichatry:Vol48.Issue4.2000:"

Copied!
9
0
0

Teks penuh

(1)

David C. Steffens, Christopher E. Byrum, Douglas R. McQuoid,

Daniel L. Greenberg, Martha E. Payne, Timothy F. Blitchington,

James R. MacFall, and K. Ranga Rama Krishnan

Background:

There is a growing literature on the

impor-tance of hippocampal volume in geriatric depression.

Methods:

We examined hippocampal volume in a group

of elderly depressed patients and a group of elderly

control subjects (

N

5

66 geriatric depressed patients and

18 elderly nondepressed control subjects) recruited

through Duke’s Mental Health Clinical Research Center

for the Study of Depression in the Elderly. The subjects

received a standardized evaluation, including a magnetic

resonance imaging scan of the brain. Patients had

unipo-lar major depression and were free of comorbid major

psychiatric illness and neurologic illness. Differences

were assessed using

t

tests and linear regression

modeling.

Results:

Accounting for the effects of age, gender, and

total brain volume, depressed patients tended to have

smaller right hippocampal volume (

p

5

.014) and left

hippocampal volume (

p

5

.073). Among depressed

pa-tients, age of onset was negatively but not significantly

related to right hippocampal volume (

p

5

.052) and to left

hippocampal volume (

p

5

.062). We noted that among

subjects with either right or left hippocampal volume of 3

mL or less, the vast majority were patients rather than

control subjects.

Conclusions:

These results support a role for

hippocam-pal dysfunction in depression, particularly in late-age

onset depression. Longitudinal studies examining both

depressive and cognitive outcomes are needed to clarify

the relationships between the hippocampus, depression,

and dementia.

Biol Psychiatry 2000;48:301–309 ©

2000

Society of Biological Psychiatry

Key Words:

Depression, hippocampus, magnetic

reso-nance imaging

Introduction

T

he role of the hippocampus in mood disorders is

unclear. There are known neural connections between

the hippocampal formation and frontal and limbic areas

that are implicated in development of depressive

symp-toms, but how and to what extent the hippocampus may

modulate mood and the pathophysiology of the stress

response is not understood (McEwen 1997; Mongeau et al

1997). Neuroimaging, particularly magnetic resonance

imaging (MRI), has been used to study the hippocampus in

populations of patients with mood disorders (Axelson et al

1993; Coffey et al 1993; Krishnan et al 1991; O’Brien et

al 1997; Sheline et al 1996). These studies have yielded

conflicting results.

Focusing on hippocampal changes in geriatric

depres-sion allows investigators to test two major hypotheses

regarding age of first onset of depression and smaller

hippocampal volumes among elderly depressives. One

hypothesis follows the stress-induced glucocorticoid

tox-icity model (McEwen 1997; Sapolsky 1993), which links

the finding of hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis

dyscon-trol leading to higher cortisol levels in depression (Carroll

et al 1981a) to subsequent glucocorticoid-induced

hip-pocampal damage (Sheline 1996). In this model, greater

length of time of depressive symptoms would predict

greater hippocampal damage. Thus, older patients with

earlier first onset of depressive symptoms should have

smaller hippocampal volumes compared with those with

later first onset of depression.

The other hypothesis follows the observation

(con-firmed by our own clinical experience) that late-onset

depression often precedes onset of dementia, particularly

Alzheimer’s disease (Jorm et al 1991; Kokmen et al 1991;

Speck et al 1995; Steffens et al 1997), which is itself

associated with hippocampal degeneration (Jack et al

1998; Laasko et al 1998). Here, a depressive syndrome

may represent the heralding sign of incident dementia, and

thus we would expect that certain individuals with

late-onset depression would have smaller hippocampal

vol-umes. Complicating hypothesis linking geriatric with

Alz-heimer’s disease is another large body of literature linking

late-onset depression with vascular brain changes

(Krish-From the Departments of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences (DCS, CEB, DRM, DLG, MEP, TFB, KRRK) and Radiology (JRM), Duke University Medical Center, Durham, North Carolina.

Address reprint requests to David C. Steffens, M.D., Assistant Professor of Psychiatry, Duke University Medical Center, Box 3903, Durham NC 27710. Received September 3, 1999; revised February 1, 2000; accepted February 7, 2000.

(2)

nan et al 1997; Soares and Mann 1997); this evidence

would suggest that cognitive decline may be a vascular

phenomenon,

possibly

with

limited

hippocampal

involvement.

Both hypotheses would thus predict smaller

hippocam-pal volumes in depressed patients versus control subjects

regardless of age of onset. If both hypotheses prove true,

then the effect of age of onset on hippocampal volume will

be determined by the relative effects of each condition,

and it is possible that one may not find any differences

when comparing early-onset with late-onset depressed

patients. Similarly, there will be no effect of age of onset

if neither model is operative. Yet it is also possible to

formulate hypotheses depending on the sample. If

late-onset depression predominates, then a significant negative

association between age of onset and hippocampal volume

may exist. Such is the case with our sample. Thus, we

predicted the following: 1) older depressed patients will

have smaller hippocampal volumes than will

nonde-pressed elderly control subjects and 2) hippocampal

vol-ume will be negatively correlated with age of onset among

the depressed group.

We sought to examine these issues in a heterogeneous

group of geriatric depressives and elderly nondepressed

controls.

Methods and Materials

Design and Sample

This study used a cross-sectional design. All subjects were participants in the NIMH Mental Health Clinical Research Center (MHCRC) for the Study of Depression in Later Life, located at Duke University, who were screened with the Center for Epidemiologic Studies-Depression Scale (CES-D; Radloff 1977). Control subjects were obtained from the Duke Aging Center Registry of elderly volunteers (60 years and older) who were free of psychiatric and neurologic illness. After complete description of the MHCRC and its procedures were explained to each subject, written informed consent was obtained.

Eligibility for patients was limited to those with CES-D scores

$16 or a diagnosis of major depression, single (incident cases) or recurrent (prevalent cases), and was restricted to patients aged 58 years or older who could speak and write English. Exclusion criteria included 1) another major psychiatric illness, such as bipolar disorder, schizophrenia, and schizoaffective disorder; 2) active alcohol or drug dependence; 3) primary neurologic illness, such as dementia, stroke, Parkinson’s disease, seizure disorder, or multiple sclerosis; 4) presence of a medical illness or medi-cation use that could affect cognitive function; 5) physical disability that precludes cognitive testing; and 6) metal in the body that precludes MRI. The MHCRC is particularly careful to assess subjects for dementia; all subjects have a clinical exami-nation by a geriatric psychiatrist, and all subjects with known or suspected dementia are excluded from the MHCRC.

At baseline, all subjects underwent a standardized section of

the MRI study (see MRI method section below). Within 60 days of the MRI, a trained interviewer administered the Duke Depres-sion Evaluation Schedule (DDES) to each enrolled subject. The DDES, a composite diagnostic interview instrument, includes sections of the NIMH Diagnostic Interview Schedule (Robins et al 1981) assessing depression (enriched with items assessing sleep problems and the clinical features of melancholia and psychosis), dysthymia, mania, and alcohol abuse or dependence. The DDES also includes the Montgomery–Asberg depression rating scale (Montgomery et al 1979), the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE; Folstein et al 1975), items assessing self-reported physical health, four subscales of the Duke Social Support Index (George et al 1989; Landerman et al 1989), and a section assessing frequency and severity of stressful life events during the year preceding the interview (Landerman et al 1989). Other clinical data, including the Carroll Rating Scale for Depression (Carroll et al 1981b), Hamilton Rating Scale for Depression (Hamilton 1960), Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer’s Disease test battery (Morris et al 1988), Hachin-ski Ischemia Scale (HachinHachin-ski et al 1975), Global Assessment Scale (Endicott et al 1976), and Cumulative Illness Rating Scale (Linn et al 1968) also were assessed at baseline.

DSM-IV diagnoses were assigned to all subjects by a consen-sus diagnostic conference, which included a board-certified or board-eligible psychiatrist, using procedures conforming to the Longitudinal, Expert and Available Data standard (Spitzer 1983) and informed by screening data, the DDES, and the clinical data listed above. Concordance between the consensus diagnoses and other diagnostic tests was moderate to high (George et al 1989).

Magnetic Resonance Imaging

MRI ACQUISITION. All subjects were screened for the presence of cardiac pacemakers, neurostimulators, metallic im-plants, metal in the orbit, aneurysm clips, or any other condition where MRI is contraindicated. Subjects were imaged with a 1.5 T whole-body MRI system (GE Medical Systems, Waukesha, WI) using the standard head (volumetric) radiofrequency coil. The scanner alignment light was used to adjust the head tilt and rotation to ensure the median was the canthomeatal line.

HIGH-RESOLUTION IMAGING FOR VOLUME MEASURE-MENT. Two dual-echo fast-spin echo acquisitions were ob-tained: one in the axial plane for morphometry for most cortical structures and a second (fast spin echo) in a coronal oblique plane for segmentation of the amygdala-hippocampus complex. The pulse sequence parameters were: TR54000 msec, TE5

30, 135 msec, 32 KHz imaging bandwidth, echo train length5

(3)

were then prescribed perpendicular to this line, covering the entire brain from just anterior to the front of the temporal lobe to posterior to the ventricles.

MR IMAGE PROCESSING. MrX Procedure (for Whole Brain) The basic segmentation protocol is a modified version of that developed by Kikinis et al (1992) and has been described previously (Byrum 1996). Gray and white contrast is often poor in MR scans of the elderly. Our seeding protocol, which identified the range of signal intensities that characterize each tissue type, was altered to address this problem. Gray matter points were selected by moving the cursor from regions of cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) on the outside of the brain to adjacent cortical gray areas while observing a two-dimensional scatter plot showing the image intensity for echo 1 graphed against the image intensity for echo 2. This eliminated the need to distin-guish the cortical gray regions from white matter, which can be nearly isointense on some scans. When the cursor first enters the cortical gray region, the location of the point in the scatter plot changes distinctly, thus identifying the gray matter region. Points for white matter seeding were selected from the anterior and posterior corpus callosum, as well as the central region of white matter tracts in each quadrant. The rationale was to exclude points that were anywhere near gray matter, and therefore least likely to be gray matter.

Once the brain was segmented into tissue types and the nonbrain tissue stripped away through a masking procedure, specific regions of interest (ROI) were assessed using tracing and connectivity functions. The cerebral hemispheres and caudate nuclei were traced and a mask was created that could be applied to the segmented brain. A connectivity function was used for quantifying the lateral ventricles.

The final step was to run a summarizing program that calculated the volume of each tissue type within the specific ROI. Volume was determined for the whole brain.

Grid Procedures (for Hippocampus) The Grid Program (developed by one of the authors, TFB) was used to quantify the left and right putamen and hippocampi. The Grid Program allows for a highly reliable, semiautomated determination of ROI volumes and is based on a manual point-counting method (MacFall et al 1994).

Our definition of the hippocampus was as follows: on each scan, we began with the most posterior coronal slice and then moved anteriorly. We began measuring the hippocampus when the pulvinar nucleus of the thalamus obscured the crura fornicis; if the crus was only obscured on one side, we only measured that side. The fimbria, which extends from the superior surface of the hippocampus across the CSF into the white matter above, was transected at its narrowest point. Along the medial border of the hippocampus, the thin strip of gray matter was cut at its narrowest point, and tracing then continued around the hip-pocampal body to the starting point. The amygdala-hiphip-pocampal transition zone appeared as a diffuse area of gray matter between the anterior portion of the hippocampus and the posterior portion of the amygdala; as with the fimbria, this area was transected at its narrowest point, which was usually found between the inferior lateral ventricles and the cistern. Continuing anteriorly, the

inferior lateral ventricles gradually shift from a vertical to a horizontal orientation but remain superior to the hippocampus. We defined the anterior border of the hippocampus as the slice on which the inferior lateral ventricles appeared horizontally without any body of gray matter visible below them. All technicians received extensive training by experienced volumet-ric analysts. Reliability was established (kappa.0.9) by repeat measurements on multiple MR scans in younger subjects before raters were approved to process study data. In addition, an ongoing reliability study was conducted to insure that the quality of volumetric analyses was maintained throughout the study. When examining older subjects, our interrater reliability was 0.79 for left hippocampus and 0.69 for right hippocampus.

Statistical Analysis

INITIAL ANALYSES. Right and left hippocampal volumes (HVs) between groups was examined usingttests for age (young vs. old with median age as cut-off), gender, race, depression versus control-subject status, and age of onset (patient group). Continuous variables (age, MMSE score, and total brain volume) were examined using analysis of variance (ANOVA) with the General Linear Models procedure (SAS Institute, Cary, NC). Differences between depressed patients and control subjects were analyzed in a series of ANOVA models (including forward selection models) with HV as the independent variable control-ling for age, gender, and total brain volume. Among the depressed group, a series of ANOVAs (including forward selec-tion) was performed to examine right and left HVs with age, gender, age of depression onset, number of previous depressive episodes, and total brain volume as independent variables. For age of onset analyses, we used an age cut-off of 45 years to distinguish between early-onset and late-onset depression, an age on which we have reported previously (Krishnan et al 1994). Analysis of variance was also used to examine differences on MMSE score within the patient group by age of onset.

POST HOC ANALYSES. We observed that in the sample of both elderly control subjects and depressed subjects, the vast majority with right or left HV below 3 mL (close to the median) were depressed patients (Figures 1 and 2). We compared group membership (control vs. patients) for subjects with small HV (3 mL or less) versus subjects with large HV (greater than 3 mL) using a chi-square test. Right and left HVs, dichotomized into small versus large groups, were then used as independent variables in a series of logistic regression models controlling for age, gender, race, and total brain volume.

Results

The Sample

(4)

control subjects were missing that variable. Figures 1 and

2 show the distribution of right and left HVs for patient

and control subjects. We determined that a cut-off below

3 mL for both right and left HVs identified a subgroup of

subjects comprised almost entirely of patient subjects.

This observation serves as the basic for post hoc analyses

(see below). There was no difference between left and

right HV scores for the sample (

p

5

.268,

t

5 2

1.112).

Bivariate Analyses

Table 2 demonstrates mean values for right and left HVs.

In bivariate analyses, uncontrolled for age, gender, and

race, depressed subjects had significantly smaller HV than

did control subjects for both right HV (

p

5

.003,

t

5

2

3.088) and left HV (

p

5

.014,

t

5 2

2.509). As shown

in the table, white subjects had larger hippocampal

vol-umes than African American subjects, and this difference

reached significance for left HV. Further analyses revealed

that African American subjects tended to be older than

white subjects, and that the proportion of women was

higher among African Americans. In subsequent analyses

that controlled for age and gender, the race variable lost

significance.

Analyses Performed within Patient and

Control Groups

In analyses using ANOVA, there was an association

among patient subjects between right HV and total brain

volume (

p

5

.007) and a nonsignificant association

between right HV and MMSE score (

p

5

.053), whereas

there was no association between right HV and age (

p

5

.097). In control subjects, there was a significant

associa-tion between right HV and total brain volume (

p

5

.033),

but no association between right HV and age (

p

5

.411) or

MMSE score (

p

5

.577). Among patients, left HV was

associated with MMSE score (

p

5

.039) and to a

nonsig-nificant degree with total brain volume (

p

5

.058), but not

with age (

p

5

.143). In the control group, left HV was

associated with total brain volume (

p

5

.048), but there

were no associations between left HV and age (

p

5

.680)

or MMSE score (

p

5

.880).

Age of Onset

Among depressed patients, age of onset was negatively but

not significantly related to right HV (

p

5

.052) and to left

HV (

p

5

.062). Number of reported episodes was

unre-lated to either right or left HV (

p

5

.999 and .942,

respectively). In the patient group, we also dichotomized

age of onset into early onset (

,

45 years,

n

5

28, mean age

of onset

5

25.6 years) and late onset (

$

45 years,

n

5

38,

mean age of onset

5

65.2 years). There were no gender

differences by age of onset, with women comprising 75%

of early-onset cases and 79% of late onset cases (

x

2

5

0.143,

p

5

.705). Mini-Mental State Examination score

was significantly associated with right HV (

p

5

.016) and

left HV (

p

5

.018) for the late-onset depressed patients,

but not for the group with early-onset depression (

p

5

.358

for right HV and

p

5

.543 for left HV).

Initial Regression Analyses

Table 3 demonstrates the results of linear regression

models with right and left HVs as the independent

vari-ables. Accounting for the effects of age, gender, and total

brain volume, depressed patients tended to have smaller

right and left HVs. In forward selection models, the

Figure 1. Numbers of subjects for each of 10 0.250-mL ranges

of right hippocampal volume.■, control subjects;h, patients.

(5)

negative association between depressed patients and HV

was significant for right HV (

p

5

.014) and close to

significance for left HV (

p

5

.073). Table 4 contains

results of linear regression models using forward selection

models demonstrating a nonsignificant negative

relation-ship between age of onset and right HV (

p

5

.153) and left

HV (

p

5

.076).

Post Hoc Analyses

We report analyses that have not undergone statistical

correction for post hoc comparison. From Figure 1, 39 of

66 (59.1%) depressed subjects and 3 of 18 (16.7%) control

subjects had a right HV less than or equal to 3 mL

[

x

2

(1)

5

10.182,

p

,

.001]. In Figure 2, 44 of 66 (66.7%)

depressed subjects and 5 of 18 (27.8%) control subjects

had a left HV less than or equal to 3 mL [

x

2

(1)

5

8.800,

p

5

.003]. In a logistic regression model (Table 5) with

right HV as a dichotomous independent variable, when we

controlled for age, gender, and total brain volume,

depres-sion status (patient vs. control subject) was a significant

predictor (odds ratio

5

7.945, CI

5

1.455– 43.400,

p

5

.017). Similarly, in a logistic regression model for left HV

(Table 5), depression status remained significant (odds

ratio

5

4.413, CI

5

1.133–17.184,

p

5

.032). C values for

the models of right HV and left HV were 0.803 and 0.779,

respectively.

We were also interested in characterizing the group of

patients with right and left HVs less than 3 mL. Bivariate

analysis using ANOVA demonstrated that for this group,

age of onset was negatively associated with right HV (

p

5

.193) with a similar trend for left HV (

p

5

.079). In a

linear regression model with age of onset, age, gender, and

total brain volume, none of the independent variables were

significant predictors of either right or left HV.

Table 1. Characteristics of the Sample

Depressed subjects (n566) Control subjects (n518) pvalue

Age, mean (SD) 71.74 (8.42) 67.11 (5.04) .005a Women,n(%) 51 (77.27) 9 (50.00) .023b Race (%)

White 56 (84.85) 17 (94.44)

African American 9 (13.64) 1 (5.56) .683c

Other 1 (1.52) 0 (0)

MMSE score, mean (SD) 27.16 (3.10) 28.83 (0.99) .0004d

at5 22.938. bx255.155.

cFisher’s exact test, 2-tailed, for white vs. African-American subjects.

dBaseline Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) score was available on 63 depressed subjects;t53.684.

Table 2. Mean Values (SD) for Right and Left Hippocampal Volumes (HVs; mL)

Right HV tscore,pa Left HV tscore,pa

Age (median age570.5)

Less than 70.5 (n5 42) 3.12 (0.41) 3.03 (0.36)

70.5 or older (n5 42) 2.97 (0.42) 1.750, .084 2.92 (0.42) 1.251, .214 Gender

Men (n5 24) 3.33 (0.42) 3.23 (0.40)

Women (n5 60) 2.93 (0.36) 4.420,,.001 2.87 (0.34) 4.195, .0001 Male patients (n 515) 3.24 (0.39) 3.15 (0.34)

Female patients (n5 51) 2.90 (0.35) 3.183, .002 2.86 (0.34) 2.911, .005 Male control subjects (n59) 3.49 (0.46) 3.38 (0.46)

Female control subjects (n 59) 3.11 (0.34) 2.022, .060 2.97 (0.34) 2.153, .047 Race

White (n 573) 3.06 (0.42) 3.01 (0.39)

African American (n 510) 2.83 (0.31) 21.744, .085 2.72 (0.29) 22.243, .028

Other (n 51) 3.78 3.27

Depression status

Patients (n5 66) 2.98 (0.39) 2.92 (0.36)

Control subjects (n518) 3.30 (0.44) 23.088, .003 3.17 (0.44) 22.509, .014 Age of onset (patients)

Onset,45 years (n528) 3.06 (0.38) 3.00 (0.35)

Onset$45 years (n538) 2.92 (0.39) 1.467, .147 2.87 (0.36) 1.437, .156

(6)

Discussion

Our study supports the hypothesis that depressed elderly

patients have smaller HV than a group of nondepressed

elderly control subjects. In some of our models controlling

for age, gender, and total brain volume, we were able to

demonstrate differences between depressed patients and

control subjects. Additionally, among depressed patients,

those with older age of onset had smaller HV. Our study

reports on 66 depressed patients and 18 elderly control

subjects; therefore, sample size considerations suggest

caution when interpreting results. Number of subjects may

also explain why some analyses yielded significant results

(particularly the logistic regression models), whereas

oth-ers demonstrated statistical trends.

Two previous studies that reported decreased HV in

major depression (Axelson et al 1993; Sheline et al 1996)

examined a wide range of age of subjects; our study

focused on geriatric patients. Axelson et al (1993) in

measuring the amygdala-hippocampal complex (AHC)

found a negative relationship between AHC volume and

age of onset of depression (significant on the left, trending

on the right) and a significant negative correlation between

left AHC volume and number of hospitalization with a

trend for trend right AHC volume. Sheline et al (1996)

reported that days of depression were negatively and

significantly associated with HV, and Axelson et al (1993)

found similar trends between duration of illness and AHC

volume. Sheline et al (1996) concluded that this was

consistent with the glucocorticoid toxicity hypothesis put

forward by Sapolsky (1993). Our findings are only mildly

supportive of that hypothesis (with a trend toward smaller

HV in patients with early-onset depression compared with

control subjects) and are rather more supportive of the

observation that late-onset depression is a risk factor for

Alzheimer’s disease (Jorm et al 1991; Kokmen et al 1991;

Table 3. Linear Regression Models for Depressed Subjects vs. Control Subjects Controlling for Age, Gender, and Total Brain Volume (Model 1) and Using Forward Selection for Significant Variables

Variable

Right hippocampal volume Left hippocampal volume

Parameter

estimate (SE) pvalue

Parameter

estimate (SE) pvalue

Model 1 (N575 subjects)

Age 20.0085 (0.0058) .149 20.0055 (0.0057) .338 Gendera

20.1306 (0.1217) .287 20.1752 (0.1202) .149 Depression statusb

20.2097 (0.1092) .059 20.1557 (0.1079) .153 Total brain volume 0.0008 (0.0003) .037 0.0005 (0.0004) .203

ModelR2 .294 .211

Model 2 (N575 subjects) Gendera

20.2855 (0.0943) .003 Depression statusb

20.2673 (0.1062) .014 20.1908 (0.1048) .073 Total brain volume 0.0010 (0.0003) .0004

ModelR2 .256 .182

aFemale subject50, male subject51. bControl subject50, depressed subject51.

Table 4. Linear Regression Models for Right and Left Hippocampal Volumes Using Forward Selection with Age of Depression Onset, Age, Gender, and Total Brain Volume as Potential Variables

Variable

Right hippocampal volume Left hippocampal volume

Parameter

estimate (SE) pvalue

Parameter

estimate (SE) pvalue

Age 20.0061 (0.0064) .341 Gendera

20.2080 (0.1364) .133 20.2803 (0.1063) .011 Age of depression onset 20.0033 (0.0023) .153 20.0036 (0.0020) .076 Total brain volume 0.0005 (0.0004) .269

ModelR2 .218 .149

Model of left hippocampal volume contains only gender and age of onset because in four-variable forward selection model, age and total brain volume both hadp..5 and were excluded.

(7)

Speck et al 1995; Steffens et al 1997), particularly with our

finding of an association between MMSE score and HV

among depressed individuals. Although we did not

mea-sure directly days of depression, our finding of a

signifi-cant negative relationship between age of onset and HV, as

well as a lack of association between HV and number of

depressive episodes, did not support those findings.

How do our findings relate to those of Sapolsky

(1993) and Sheline et al (1996)? Although they only

mildly support their findings, glucocorticoid function in

geriatric depression may express itself in a different

manner from other clinical populations. For example,

the severe, late-onset depression experienced by many

of our subjects may be associated with greater

hypotha-lamic-pituitary-adrenal axis dysregulation or with more

comorbid medical illness. Such differences may relate

to severity of illness or the underlying pathophysiology

of illness. Thus, higher glucocorticoid exposure may be

related to the intensity and biology of the illness in

addition to its duration. In support of this hypothesis (in

results not shown), we found that patients with severe

depression had smaller HV compared with other

de-pressed individuals, with the difference on the left being

greater, but these results were not statistically

signifi-cant. Axelson et al (1993) found no relationship

be-tween severity of depression and AHC volume.

Alternatively, it may be that depressed subjects in

this sample are comprised of patients with either

late-onset preclinical dementia or early-late-onset recurrent

ma-jor depression. Those with preclinical dementia may not

be experiencing any glucocorticoid-induced

neurotox-icity, but they may have experienced a much larger

decrease in hippocampal volume as a consequence of

the dementing process. Simply put, the changes in

hippocampal volume may be greater in late-onset

de-pression compared with early-onset dede-pression because

the pathologic processes associated with late-onset

depression exert greater hippocampal effects than does

glucocorticoid neurotoxicity in patients with early-onset

depression. Clearly, more investigations using larger

samples of patients are needed to clarify the relationship

between duration of illness, severity of illness, stress

level, and hippocampal volume.

One potential limitation of our study was the timing of

the MRI scans in relation to entry into the study. With our

protocol, patients had a 2-month window upon entry into

the study to have a MRI scan. If HV is associated with

state effects of depression (e.g., higher cortisol levels),

then patients who underwent an MRI scan when they were

most acutely depressed might have smaller HV than those

who were unable to have an MRI upon entry to the study.

Coffey et al (1991), however, found no difference between

baseline, 2-week and 6-month volumes of the

amygdala-hippocampal complex in depressed patients receiving

electroconvulsive therapy. Thus, it is doubtful that over

the course of 2 months, much change in HV should occur.

We also may face limitations from our scanning

method. We used 3-mm MRI slices that may have limited

our ability to define the hippocampus or differentiate it

from surrounding structures. It also may have led to partial

volume artifacts. Lack of clarity between of hippocampal

boundaries in these older subjects may have contributed to

the suboptimal interrater reliabilities.

Caution is also warranted in the interpretation of our

post hoc results. We did not perform a statistical correction

to account for post hoc analysis. It became clear upon

inspection of the data presented in Figures 1 and 2 that

further analyses using 3 mL as a cutoff might yield

interesting results. We then performed four sets of

analy-ses for right and left HVs. The initial chi-square analyanaly-ses

of depression status by volume were highly significant and

remain significant after post hoc statistical correction. The

Table 5. Logistic Regression Models for Controlling for Age, Gender, Depression Status, and Total Brain Volume (Model 1) and with Forward Regression (Model 2)

Variable

Right hippocampal volume

Left hippocampal volume

Model 1: full model (n575 subjects)

Age 1.021 (0.951–1.096) 1.023 (0.952–1.099) Gendera 2.900 (0.647–13.006) 7.145 (1.545–33.042) Depression statusb 7.945 (1.455– 43.400) 4.413 (1.133–17.184) Total brain volume 0.996 (0.991–1.001) 1.002 (0.997–1.006)

Model c value .803 .779

Model 2: Forward selection (n 575 subjects)

Gendera 5.263 (1.662–16.660)

Depression statusb 9.282 (1.777– 48.493) 4.736 (1.255–17.870) Total brain volume 0.995 (0.990 – 0.999)

Model c value .780 .739

Odds ratios with confidence intervals for right and left hippocampal volumes dichotomized at,3 mL and$3 mL.

(8)

subsequent logistic models (

p

5

.017 for right HV,

p

5

0.032 for left HV) may lose significance. Future studies

that dichotomize the volume of the hippocampus should

employ larger samples of depressed patients to overcome

post hoc correction.

Clinical longitudinal research on hippocampal

vol-ume and function is needed to address the role of the

hippocampus in affective disorders. Such studies will

need to focus on both depression and cognitive

out-comes. In particular, studies will need to examine HV in

older depressed patients who subsequently develop

dementia. Alzheimer’s disease is associated with

hip-pocampal atrophy, whereas vascular dementia may not

be. Thus, the study of hippocampal volume in geriatric

depression may inform the relationship between

depres-sion and Alzheimer’s disease.

This study was supported by NIMH Grants Nos. P 30 MH40159, R01 MH54846, and K07 MH01367. Presented at the Annual Meeting of the American Association for Geriatric Psychiatry, March 13–17, 1999.

References

Axelson DA, Doraiswamy PM, McDonald WM, Boyko OB, Tupler LA, Patterson LJ, et al (1993): Hypercortisolemia and hip-pocampal changes in depression.Psychiatry Res47:163–173. Byrum CE, MacFall JR, Charles HC, Chitilla VR, Boyko OB,

Upchurch L, et al (1996): Accuracy and reproducibility of brain and tissue volumes using a magnetic resonance segmen-tation method.Psychiatry Res67:215–234.

Carroll BJ, Feinberg M, Greden JF, Tarika J, Albala AA, Haskett RF, et al (1981a): A specific laboratory test for the diagnosis of melancholia. Standardization, validation, and clinical util-ity.Arch Gen Psychiatry38:15–22.

Carroll BJ, Feinberg M, Smouse PE, Rawson SG, Greden JF (1981b): The Carroll Rating Scale for Depression. I. Develop-ment, reliability, and validation.Br J Psychiatry138:194 –200. Coffey CE, Weiner RD, Djang WT, Figiel GS, Soady SA, Patterson LJ, et al (1991): Brain anatomic effects of electro-convulsive therapy. A prospective magnetic resonance imag-ing study.Arch Gen Psychiatry48:1013–1021.

Coffey CE, Wilkinson WE, Weiner RD, Parashos IA, Djang WT, Webb MC, et al (1993): Quantitative cerebral anatomy in depression. A controlled magnetic resonance imaging study.

Arch Gen Psychiatry50:7–16.

Endicott J, Spitzer RL, Fleiss JL, Cohen J (1976): The Global Assessment Scale: A procedure for measuring overall sever-ity of psychiatric disturbance.Arch Gen Psychiatry33:766 – 771.

Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR (1975): “Mini-mental state.” A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician.J Psychiatr Res12:189 –198. George LK, Blazer DG, Hughes DC, Fowler N (1989): Social

support and the outcome of major depression.Br J Psychiatry

154:478 – 485.

Hachinski VC, Iliff LD, Zilhka E, Du Boulay GH, McAllister

VL, Marshall J, et al (1975): Cerebral blood flow in dementia.

Arch Neurol32:632– 637.

Hamilton M (1960): A rating scale for depression. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry23:56 – 61.

Jack CR Jr, Petersen RC, Xu YC, O’Brien PC, Waring SC, Tangalos EG, et al (1998): Hippocampal atrophy and apoli-poprotein E genotype are independently associated with Alzheimer’s disease.Ann Neurol43:303–310.

Jorm AF, van Duijn CM, Chandra V, Fratiglioni L, Graves AB, Heyman A, et al (1991): Psychiatric history and related exposures as risk factors for Alzheimer’s disease: A collab-orative re-analysis of case-control studies. Int J Epidemiol

20(suppl 2):S43–S47.

Kikinis R, Shenton ME, Gerig G, Martin J, Anderson M, Metcalf D, et al (1992): Routine quantitative analysis of brain and cerebrospinal fluid spaces with MR imaging.J Magn Reson Imaging2:619 – 629.

Kokmen E, Beard CM, Chandra V, Offord KP, Schoenberg BS, Ballard DJ (1991): Clinical risk factors for Alzheimer’s disease: A population-based case-control study. Neurology

41:1393–1397.

Krishnan KR, Doraiswamy PM, Figiel GS, Husain MM, Shah SA, Na C, et al (1991): Hippocampal abnormalities in depression.J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci3:387–391. Krishnan KR, Hays JC, Blazer DG (1997): MRI-defined vascular

depression.Am J Psychiatry154:497–501.

Krishnan KRR, Ritchie JC Jr, Tupler LA, McDonald WM, Knight D, Nemeroff CB, et al (1994): Apolipoprotein E epsilon 4.Neurology44:2420 –2421.

Laakso MP, Soininen H, Partanen K, Lehtovirta M, Hallikainen M, Hanninen T, et al (1998): MRI of the hippocampus in Alzheimer’s disease: sensitivity, specificity, and analysis of the incorrectly classified subjects.Neurobiol Aging19:23–31. Landerman R, George LK, Campbell RT, Blazer DG (1989): Alternative models of the stress buffering hypothesis.Am J Community Psychol17:626 – 642.

Linn BS, Linn MW, Gurel L (1968): Cumulative illness rating scale.J Am Geriatr Soc16:622– 626.

MacFall JR, Byrum CE, Parashos I, Early B, Charles HC, Chittilla V, et al (1994): Relative accuracy and reproducibility of regional MRI brain volumes for point-counting methods.

Psychiatry Res55:167–177.

McEwen BS (1997): Possible mechanisms for atrophy of the human hippocampus.Mol Psychiatry2:255–262.

Mongeau R, Blier P, de Montigny C (1997): The serotonergic and noradrenergic systems of the hippocampus: Their inter-actions and the effects of antidepressant treatments.Brain Res Brain Res Rev23:145–195.

Montgomery SA, Asberg M (1979): A new depression scale designed to be sensitive to change.Br J Psychiatry134:382– 389.

Morris JC, Mohs RC, Rogers H, Fillenbaum G, Heyman A (1988): Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer’s Disease (CERAD) clinical and neuropsychological assess-ment of Alzheimer’s disease.Psychopharmacol Bull24:641– 652.

(9)

depres-sion, vascular dementia and other causes of cognitive impair-ment.Psychol Med27:1267–1275.

Radloff LS (1977): The CES-D scale: A self-report depression scale for research in the general population.Appl Psychol Measures1:385– 401.

Robins LN, Helzer JE, Croughan J, Ratcliff KS (1981): National Institute of Mental Health Diagnostic Interview Schedule: Its history, characteristics, and validity. Arch Gen Psychiatry

38:381–389.

Sapolsky RM (1993): Potential behavioral modification of glu-cocorticoid damage to the hippocampus. Behav Brain Res

57:175–182.

Sheline YI (1996): Hippocampal atrophy in major depression: A result of depression-induced neurotoxicity. Mol Psychiatry

1:298 –299.

Sheline YI, Wang PW, Gado MH, Csernansky JG, Vannier MW (1996): Hippocampal atrophy in recurrent major depression.

Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A93:3908 –3913.

Soares JC, Mann JJ (1997): The anatomy of mood disorders— review of structural neuroimaging studies. Biol Psychiatry

41:86 –106.

Speck CE, Kukull WA, Brenner DE, Bowen JD, McCormick WC, Teri L, et al (1995): History of depression as a risk factor for Alzheimer’s disease.Epidemiology6:366 –369.

Spitzer RL (1983): Psychiatric diagnosis: Are clinicians still necessary?Compr Psychiatry24:399 – 411.

Gambar

Table 2 demonstrates mean values for right and left HVs.In bivariate analyses, uncontrolled for age, gender, and�race, depressed subjects had significantly smaller HV thandid control subjects for both right HV (p � .003, t �3.088) and left HV (p � .014, t
Table 1. Characteristics of the Sample
Table 3. Linear Regression Models for Depressed Subjects vs. Control Subjects Controlling forAge, Gender, and Total Brain Volume (Model 1) and Using Forward Selection for SignificantVariables
Table 5. Logistic Regression Models for Controlling for Age, Gender, Depression Status, andTotal Brain Volume (Model 1) and with Forward Regression (Model 2)

Referensi

Dokumen terkait

Pada tahap akhir adalah penampungan semen dengan cara pejantan ditunggu sampai mau menaiki betina, setelah pejantan menaiki betina dan melakukan ereksi gland penis maka gland

Sebuah skripsi yang diajukan untuk memenuhi salah satu syarat memperoleh gelar. Sarjana Pendidikan pada Departemen Pendidikan Sejarah

Pada pengaturan Replace color ini, silahkan sobat klik Ikon eyedropper too kemudian klik kan pada warna background yang akan di ganti2. Ganti juga warna Result

Keadaan tersebut menimbulkan berbagai reaksi dari masyarakat Sumedang, diantaranya banyak dari masyarakat Sumedang yang menolak kehadiran gerakan DI/TII dan tidak

Dalam proses kritik eksternal ini, peneliti beranggapan bahwa sumber dari Arsip tentang DI/TII thn 1950 ( Laporan PDM Sumedang, Jawa Barat) ini merupakan sumber

Sekolah Menjalin kemitraan dalam rangka perlindungan dan pengelolaan lingkungan hidup dengan berbagai pihak (masyarakat, pemerintah, swasta, media, sekolah lain)... PENGELOLAAN

DILEMA MASYARAKAT SUMEDANG TERHADAP KEHADIRAN GERAKAN DI/TII TAHUN 1950-1962.. Universitas Pendidikan Indonesia | repository.upi.edu | perpustakaan.upi.edu

PROGRAM ADIWIYATA adalah program untuk mewujudkan sekolah yang peduli dan berbudaya lingkungan.. SEKOLAH ADIWIYATA : adalah sekolah peduli dan