• Tidak ada hasil yang ditemukan

Directory UMM :Data Elmu:jurnal:B:Biological Psichatry:Vol48.Issue10.2000:

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2017

Membagikan "Directory UMM :Data Elmu:jurnal:B:Biological Psichatry:Vol48.Issue10.2000:"

Copied!
8
0
0

Teks penuh

(1)

Releasing Hormone in Child Depression

Ronald E. Dahl, Boris Birmaher, Douglas E. Williamson, Lorah Dorn, James Perel,

Joan Kaufman, David A. Brent, David A. Axelson, and Neal D. Ryan

Background:

This study examined growth hormone (GH)

response to growth hormone–releasing hormone (GHRH)

in a large sample of depressed children compared with

normal control children. Within-subject comparisons were

also performed in control subjects to examine test–retest

reliability and in depressed children comparing episode

versus clinical recovery.

Methods:

The sample included depressed children (n

5

82) and normal control children (n

5

55) group-matched

for age, gender, and pubertal status; the mean ages were

11.2

6

1.7 and 11.2

6

1.8 years, respectively. We gave

GHRH (0.1 mcg/Kg) at 9

AM

, and serum GH levels were

determined every 15 min from

2

30 min through

1

90 min

of the GHRH infusion. A subgroup of normal control

subjects (n

5

11) repeated the protocol for test–retest

reliability within a 2-month interval. A subgroup of

depressed children (n

5

20) were restudied off all

medi-cations following full clinical remission from depression.

Results:

The mean GH response to GHRH was

signifi-cantly lower in the depressed group (8.7 ng/mL

6

SEM

0.9) compared with normal control children [12.2 ng/

mL

6

SEM 1.3;

t(135)

5

2.59,

p

5

.01 effect size 0.44].

The test–retest reliability of GH response to GHRH was

stable (intraclass correlation

5

.93 for mean post-GH).

The GH response to GHRH remained low in subjects

restudied during clinical remission from depression.

Conclusions:

Depressed children show low GH response

to GHRH. The measure appears to be reliable, and the low

GH response continues following clinical remission.

Fur-ther studies are needed to explore the mechanism and

relative specificity of this finding.

Biol Psychiatry 2000;

48:981–988 ©

2000 Society of Biological Psychiatry

Key Words:

Depression, growth hormone, growth

hor-mone–releasing

hormone,

development,

children,

adolescents

Introduction

P

rogress in understanding affective disorders over the

past decade increasingly has focused attention on

questions of maturational influences on biological systems

of interest. There has been considerable interest in 1) the

effects of early adverse experiences on later expression of

affective dysregulation, 2) the increasing rates of

depres-sion during adolescent development, 3) the emergence of

gender differences in rates of depression during adolescent

development, and 4) changes across childhood,

adoles-cence, adulthood, and geriatrics in specific domains, such

as symptom profile, clinical course, genetic influences,

and treatment response. Broadly speaking, this work has

yielded some areas of maturational continuity, as well as

areas of developmental variation (Angold et al 1998;

Birmaher et al 1996a, 1996b; Garber et al 1993;

Har-rington et al 1994; Kovacs 1996; Puig-Antich et al 1993;

Rao et al 1995; Ryan et al 1987).

This theme of both similarities and differences across

child, adolescent, and adult depression also has been

evident in psychobiologic studies. For example, HPA-axis

alterations

and

electroencephalogram

(EEG)

sleep

changes associated with depression show considerable

maturational variance (Dahl 1996; Dahl et al 1991a,

1991b, 1992, 1994, 1996; Emslie et al 1994). In contrast,

low growth hormone (GH) response to pharmacologic

challenges seen in association with depression has shown

relative continuity across development, including

de-pressed samples ranging from prepubertal children

through old age (Jarrett et al 1990, 1994; Lesch et al 1987,

1989; Meyer et al 1991; Puig-Antich et al 1984c, 1984d;

Ryan et al 1988, 1994; Siever et al 1992).

There is also preliminary evidence that this low GH

persists after clinical recovery (in a medication-free state)

in depressed children (Puig-Antich et al 1984d) and

following treatment for depression in adults (Jarrett et al

1994).

Recently, our research group has found that low GH

response to growth hormone–releasing hormone is also

evident in children and adolescents with no personal

history of depression but with high rates of affective

From the Department of Psychiatry, University of Pittsburgh School of Medicine,

Western Psychiatric Institute and Clinic, Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania (RED, BB, DEW, LD, JP, DAB, DAA, NDR) and the Department of Psychiatry, Yale University School of Medicine, New Haven, Connecticut (JK).

Address reprint requests to Ronald E. Dahl, M.D., University of Pittsburgh Medical Center, Western Psychiatric Institute and Clinic, 3811 O’Hara Street, Pitts-burgh PA 15213.

Received January 8, 2000; revised May 1, 2000; accepted May 18, 2000.

© 2000 Society of Biological Psychiatry 0006-3223/00/$20.00

(2)

illness in their families (Birmaher et al 2000). These

results have been considered within an emerging model

based on the hypothesis that low GH response to GHRH

represents a stable

trait

marker for affective illness—a

marker evident before the onset of depression, persisting

across episode and recovery, and independent of age and

maturational changes.

The studies reported in this paper address the basic

foundation for further investigations of this model,

focus-ing on three specific aims: 1) to extend earlier findfocus-ings of

low GH response to GHRH by comparing a large sample

of depressed children and well-matched control subjects

with careful assessment of psychiatric and family history

data, 2) to examine the

reliability

of GH responses to

physiologic stimuli (within-subject test–retest reliability),

and 3) to examine GH responses after clinical recovery

within a subset of depressed children studied initially

during a depressive episode.

Methods and Materials

Phase 1 and Phase 2 Studies

The data described in this paper include GH data collected in two similar studies. The research facility, most of the personnel, GH assay procedures, and design of the study up to and including the GHRH test were identical. Phase 1 of the study included a larger number of neuroendocrine tests after the GHRH test; a later phase of the study had a shorter neuroendocrine protocol. The GHRH data from n 5 36 children with major depressive disorder (MDD) n5 18 normal control subjects from phase 1 studies were included in a preliminary report (Ryan et al 1994). All of the phase 2 data are new, and there is no overlap with any previously reported GH data. The methods for the GHRH test measures and protocol were identical across studies, and analyses were performed showing that there was no effect of phase on any of the GH measures being examined, thus the data were com-bined across phase 1 and phase 2 studies to maximize the power to examine covariates and subgroup effects.

Recruitment and Inclusion–Exclusion Criteria

Depressed children were recruited from the inpatient and outpa-tient clinics at Western Psychiatric Institute and Clinic, Univer-sity of Pittsburgh Medical Center. Normal control children were recruited through advertisement and personal contacts. Informed consent to participate in the study was obtained in accordance with the University of Pittsburgh Institutional Review Board guidelines. Inclusion criteria common for all subjects were that they be 7 to 15 years of age and medically healthy. Children in the depressed cohort were required to meet DSM-III-R criteria for MDD. Additional exclusion criteria for the MDD cohorts only included 1) concurrent DSM-III-R diagnosis of anorexia nervosa, bulimia nervosa, autism, schizoaffective disorder, or schizophrenia and 2) MDD chronologically secondary to conduct disorder.

For the normal control subjects only, additional inclusion

criteria included 1) no lifetime history of any psychiatric disorder and 2) low familial risk for affective illness. In this study, low familial risk for affective disorder was operationally defined as having no first-degree relative with a lifetime episode of any affective disorder or schizophrenia spectrum illness; no second-degree relative with a lifetime history of recurrent, bipolar, or psychotic depression, schizoaffective disorder, or schizophrenia; and no more than 20% of second-degree relatives with a single episode of MDD.

Exclusion criteria for both groups were 1) significant medical illnesses, 2) medications (except acetaminophen) within 2 weeks of the study, 3) inordinate fear of needles, 4) obesity (weight greater than 150% of ideal body weight) or severe growth failure (weight or height less than 3% of the National Health Statistic Curve), and 5) mental retardation or the presence of a specific learning disability.

DIAGNOSTIC ASSIGNMENT. Diagnostic assessments of the

depressed cohorts were completed by research assistants who administered the Present Episode (Chambers et al 1985) and Epidemiologic (Orvaschel and Puig-Antich 1987) versions of the semistructured diagnostic interview, the Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia for School-Aged Children (K-SADS). The diagnosis of MDD was made using DSM-III-R criteria, with the presence of all positive symptoms confirmed by a child psychiatrist or psychologist. For normal control subjects, if an initial telephone screen of the family indicated preliminary eligibility and interest, the child’s lifetime history of psychiatric symptomatology was assessed using only the K-SADS-E (Or-vaschel and Puig-Antich 1987). All symptom ratings were made by interviewing the parent(s) first, then interviewing the child. All children who participated in the study were reinterviewed using a short version of the K-SADS at the time of the psychobiological studies to confirm presence of depressive symptomatology.

All interviews were carried out by trained research clinicians blind to the subject’s clinical status under the supervision of child psychiatrists (DA, BB) or a child psychologist (JK). Interrater reliability for all diagnoses for our studies has beenk$ .70. Socioeconomic status (SES) was measured using the Hollings-head four-factor index (HollingsHollings-head 1975).

GHRH TEST. Following a visit and orientation to the

labo-ratory, children and family members arrived in the lab to begin the psychobiological study at 5 PM. The sleep–neuroendocrine laboratory is furnished with many age-appropriate materials, including books, art supplies, board games, entertainment videos, and computer games. Children are given considerable individual attention from staff with years of experience conducting biolog-ical studies with children. On a rating scale completed immedi-ately after finishing the psychobiological studies, the majority of children who participated in this and other studies conducted in the lab rated the experience as “very positive” (Dahl and Ryan 1999).

(3)

following morning, baseline blood samples for GH were ob-tained through the indwelling catheter at 8:30, 8:45, and 9:00AM. Immediately following the 9:00AMdraw, children received an

infusion of 1.0 mcg/kg of GHRH over 2 min. There were no significant side effects of any sort related to the GHRH test. Children generally showed no signs of behavioral reaction of any kind. A physician was in attendance throughout the procedure. Following GHRH administration, blood samples for GH were obtained every 15 min for a total of 90 min. The child remained fasting from bedtime (of the previous evening) until completion of the GHRH test.

BLOOD SAMPLES AND GH ASSAYS. Blood samples were collected in plastic tubes containing edetic acid (EDTA) then centrifuged immediately in a refrigerated centrifuge. Plasma was separated and stored at 280°C until assayed. Human growth hormone levels were measured on a 50mL plasma sample, run in duplicate. A modified version of the double antibody 125I

radioimmunoassay procedure developed by Diagnostic Products Corporation (Los Angeles) was used for these determinations. To increase the sensitivity of the assay to 0.5 ng/mL of growth hormone from that of the straight kit procedure, the amount of antibody was decreased, and a longer, sequential incubation of the antibody and radiolabeled antigen was used. Subjects’ duplicates with a coefficient of variation exceeding 5.0% were retested. The range for the intraassay variation of subjects’ sample duplicates was 0.00 to 4.98% CV, with a mean of 0.87% CV. Interassay variation ranged from 20.15% CV (mean of 2.47 ng/mL) to 13.68% CV (mean of 12.31 ng/mL).

Statistical Analyses

Sample characteristics were compared using analyses of vari-ance,x2, and Fisher’s exact tests as appropriate. Problem with missing hormonal data were minimal, with only a few subjects missing one or two data points. Analyzing the data with and without linear interpolation to fill in the occasional missing values yielded similar results, therefore only the interpolated data are reported. Before conducting any analyses, summary variables for the hormonal measures and clinical rating scales were examined for normality using the Shapiro and Wilks’W statistic. The data were subject to log or square root transformation where significantly nonnormal distributions were found. In cases where no transformation normalized the data, nonparametric tests were used. The following summary values were used to characterize children’s GH responses: baseline, peak, and mean post-GH. The baseline values were computed by determining the mean of the three GH specimens taken at230,215, and 0 min preinfusion. The peak value represents the highest value postinfusion, and the mean post-GH values were the average of samples from115 through190 min. (The GH levels by 90 min were back to a level that was not significantly different from baseline.) A comparison using total GH, as computed by determining the area under the curve with all GH values obtained from 0 to 90 min post-GH using the trapezoidal rule revealed identical group comparisons; only the mean values are reported here.

Reliability Study

To assess the test–retest reliability of the GHRH test, the protocol was repeated in 11 normal control subjects within 1 to 2 months after the initial study.

Recovery Study

In the clinical follow-up of the depressed sample, subjects were invited to participate in a repeat study when they had recovered from the depressive episode. To be considered recovered, de-pressed children were required to be fully remitted for 8 consecutive weeks (Frank et al 1991) and medication free for at least 8 weeks. At the time of these analyses,n520 subjects had been restudied in the same protocol following remission and had their data compared with the findings from GH data obtained in the depressed state.

Results

There were no significant group differences in age,

gen-der, body mass index, or Tanner stage of sexual

matura-tion (Table 1). The groups were significantly different in

SES [39.2

6

SD 13.9 vs. 47.8

6

SD 11.1;

t(127)

5

3.68,

p

5

.0003].

Because SES was significantly different between MDD

and control groups, this variable was examined in relation

to the summary GH measures and was not found to be a

significant covariate for any of the GH measures. There

were also no significant differences in GH responses

between male and female subjects in any of these

analy-ses; thus, male and females subjects were combined for all

comparisons. Analyses examining pubertal status (Tanner

stage) also showed no significant effect on GH response to

GHRH.

Depressed versus Normal Control Subjects

Baseline GH measures (

2

30

2

15, and 0 min before

GHRH infusion) were not different between groups (0.4

ng/mL

6

SEM 0.05 vs. 0.4

6

0.05). Following infusion of

GHRH, the depressed subjects (n

5

82) secreted

signif-Table 1. Demographic Characteristics in Depressed and Low-Risk Children

Variable

MDD (n 5 82)

NC

(n 5 55) Statistic

p value

Age 11.261.7 11.261.8 t(135)50.11 ns Gender (F/M) 29/53 22/33 x250.14 ns Race Fisher exact test (W/B/O) 66/15/1 49/3/3 .0314 SES 39.2613.9 47.8611.1 t(127)53.68 .0003 BMI 19.463.6 18.864.1 t(135)51.20 ns

(4)

icantly less GH than control subjects (n

5

55) as

measured by peak levels [13.9 ng/mL

6

SEM 1.4 vs. 19.5

ng/mL

6

SEM 2.1;

t

(135)

5

2.6,

p

5

.02], and mean

levels postinfusion [8.7 ng/mL

6

SEM 0.9 vs. 12.2

6

1.3;

t(135)

5

2.6,

p

5

.01; Figure 1 and Table 2]. The

effect size for the between-group differences in GH

response based on the entire sample (n

5

82 MDD

subjects;

n

5

55 control subjects) was 0.44; the effect

size when examining only the independent replication

sample (

n

5

46 MDD subjects;

n

5

37 control subjects)

was 0.39.

GH Results in Relation to Severity and Other

Clinical Symptoms

We examined GH response to GHRH in relation to clinical

symptoms, including prior episodes of depression (15% of

the sample had a history of a past episode of depression),

duration of depression (mean duration was 42.4

6

36.8

weeks), severity of depression (mean depression 12 score

from the K-SADS was 31.4

6

5.7), and comorbid

symp-toms of anxiety sympsymp-toms (53% of the depressed sample

had current or past anxiety). None of these analyses

showed any significant effect on GH response to GHRH

within the MDD sample.

Test–Retest Reliability

The GH response to GHRH showed an intraclass

correla-tion (ICC)

5

.89 (peak) and

r

5

.93 (mean post) within

the 11 subjects who underwent repeat testing within 2

months (Figure 2).

GH Responses following Clinical Recovery

On restudy in a medication-free state, GH responses were

not significantly different than their previous values

ob-tained during the episode of MDD; mean GH post-GHRH

was 9.21

6

10.82 versus 8.10

6

7.61 [t

(19)

5

0.36, ns]

and peak GH post-GHRH was 13.40

6

14.78 versus

13.72

6

12.82 [t(19)

5

0.07, ns] in depressed and

remitted states respectively. As shown in Figure 3, the GH

response in the MDD-recovered group (n

5

20)

re-mained significantly lower than the GH response in the

control group.

GH Responses in Relation to

Hypothalmic–Pituitary–Adrenal Axis Measures

The subjects in this study had also undergone

measure-ments of cortisol and corticotropin (baseline and following

corticotropin-releasing

hormone

challenge);

analyses

comparing summary hypothalmic–pituitary–adrenal axis

Figure 1. Growth hormone response to growth

hormone–releas-ing hormone (GHRH) in children and adolescents with major depressive disorder (F;n5 82) compared to low-risk normal control subjects (E; n 5 55). Time 0 corresponds to the infusion of GHRH. Values are in ng/mL and represent the means6SEMs.

Figure 2. Growth hormone response to growth hormone–releas-ing hormone in low-risk normal control subjects restudied for test–retest reliability. Values are in ng/mL and represent the means6SEMs.F, time 1 (n 5 11);E, time 2 (n 5 11).

Table 2. Demographic Characteristics and Growth Hormone Response to GHRH and during Baseline in Depressed and Low-Risk Children

Variable

MDD (n 582)

NC

(n 5 55) Statistic p value

GHRH

Baseline 0.460.05 0.460.05 t13550.32 ns

Postinfusion 8.760.9 12.261.3 t13552.59 .0107

Peak postinfusion 13.961.4 19.562.1 t13552.55 .0119

All between-group comparisons on growth hormone were done using log-transformed growth hormone values; reported growth hormone values are means6

(5)

measures and GH measures revealed no significant

corre-lations within the MDD or control samples.

Discussion

Because of the challenges in recruitment and performing

biological studies in children, psychobiological studies in

child psychiatry typically entail small clinical and control

samples. There is also an inherent bias toward publishing

studies that have found group differences, compared with

small-sample “negative” studies that are less likely to be

written-up and published. This classic “file-drawer

prob-lem” is particularly concerning in the arena of child

psychiatry.

Large controlled studies and independent replications

are infrequent but essential to moving the field forward.

This article establishes a large, independent replication of

low GH response to pharmacologic stimulation associated

with child depression, which was first reported in

Puig-Antich’s seminal papers (Puig-Antich et al 1984a, 1984b,

1984c, 1984d).

By examining a large sample of depressed children

(n

5

82) selected over a 10-year interval in a

well-controlled study, the data in this article establish strong

support for the hypothesis that low GH response to GHRH

represents a neuroendocrine marker of affective illness in

children. Our study also establishes good test-retest

reli-ability for the measure, and demonstrates continuity of low

GH response to GHRH in depressed children in clinical

remission.

There are also several limitations to these data to be

considered. One issue that has not been addressed is the

specificity

of the finding to depression. It is possible that

low GH is associated more generally with psychiatric or

emotional difficulties in childhood. Few studies have

examined GH responses to pharmacologic challenge in

other child psychiatric disorders. A recent study in

chil-dren with anxiety using a clonidine challenge (Sallee et al

1998) reported

increased

GH responses in the anxious

children compared with control subjects. The sample in

that study also included children with

obsessive-compul-sive disorder, as well as other anxiety disorders.

Further-more, studies in adult samples have generally reported low

GH responses to several types of pharmacologic

chal-lenge, in depression

and

anxiety disorders (Coplan et al

1997; Holsboer 1995; Uhde et al 1986) including

gener-alized anxiety disorder (Abelson et al 1991) social phobia

(Tancer et al 1993), and panic disorder (Charney et al

1987; Coplan et al 1997, in press; Nutt 1989; Rapaport et

al 1989). Many of the pharmacologic challenges in adult

studies, however, (particularly clonidine and yohimbe

challenges) were based on the assumption that researchers

were evaluating the integrity of noradrenergic sensitivity

and that GH was simply a marker.

Focusing more specifically on GHRH stimulation

stud-ies in psychiatry, a critical review by (Skare et al 1994)

discussed four replications showing low GH response to

GHRH in adult depression, as well as two failures to

replicate. These authors summarized the promising results

but raised several concerns, including 1) the variability of

GH response among control subjects, 2) the need to

consider the age and gender of the subjects, 3) the lack of

uniformity of procedures and measures, and 4) absence of

data in adult studies regarding the test–retest reliability

within subjects. It is important to note that among the

controlled studies reviewed in that paper, the sample sizes

ranged from

n

5

7 to

n

5

19 subjects with depression

and the total number of depressed adults is only

n

5

80

(combining all studies). In contrast, our study contained

n

5

82 depressed children and

n

5

55 control subjects.

In addition, we performed reliability tests, examined the

influences of age and gender carefully, and used uniform

procedures based on previous studies.

One study of GHRH in depressed adults (Gann et al

1995) published after the Skare review reported no

differ-ences in GH response to GHRH; however, the sample

contained only

n

5

12 MDD subjects and

n

5

12 control

subjects and reported the GH was 645

6

770 (area under

the curve) in the MDD subjects and 1031

6

279 in the

control subjects. Thus, that “negative” paper actually

found lower GH response to GHRH in the MDD group

with an effect size of 0.67 (comparable to our results) and

likely failed to find significant group differences because

of the small samples.

(6)

Effects on Growth

The functional significance of lower GH response

regard-ing growth processes is unclear. There were no significant

differences in height or weight between groups, and

preliminary measures of height velocity were not different

between groups. It is possible that any difference in GH

regulation in response to GHRH challenge is not reflected

in overall GH production or IGF-1 levels, and that it does

not have any functional impact on growth. On the other

hand, these effects may be subtle and below the threshold

of detection over short intervals of time because Pine et al

reported a slight but statistically significant reduction in

expected adult height in the follow-up of children with

anxiety disorders, but only in the females (Pine et al 1996).

Functional Significance and Mechanism of Action

Since our finding is stable following clinical recovery, one

possibility is that this represents some type of early “scar”

marker related to having had a depressive episode;

how-ever, other work by our research group (Birmaher et al

2000) shows evidence of low GH in children at high

familial risk of depression but without any clinical signs of

depression. In that study, the mean GH response in the

nondepressed “high-risk” sample (n

5

64) was 8.6

ng/mL

6

1.1, similar to the MDD sample (

n

5

82) 8.7

ng/mL

6

0.9 with both groups significantly different from

the normal control subjects without family loading for

depression (n

5

55) at 12.2 ng/mL

6

1.3. Taken

together, these data are consistent with a model that low

GH response to GHRH represents a “trait” marker

asso-ciated with the risk or vulnerability toward depression.

Further, the absence of any significant relationship of GH

response to severity of depression or duration of

depres-sion is also consistent with the concept of a trait marker.

Another possibility is that early stress or adverse social

experiences early in life may alter GH regulation

and

confer greater risk of depression. Some animal studies

have found low GH response in monkeys and rodents that

were exposed to maternal separation early in life.

Cham-poux et al 1989 reported low GH responses to

pharmaco-logic challenge in nursery-reared infant rhesus macaques

at 37 days of age compared with mother-reared control

subjects (the nursery-reared monkeys also showed higher

rates of depressive like behaviors later in life (Champoux

et al 1989). Coplan et al (1998), studying bonnet macaques

raised in variable foraging demand (VFD) conditions

(Rosenblum et al 1994), found low GH response to

clonidine infusion in the VFD offspring that showed high

rates of anxious social behaviors and low exploration.

These VFD offspring were also found to have elevated

cerebrospinal fluid somatostatin (GH-inhibiting peptide)

(Coplan et al 1998). Most recently Plotsky’s research

group (Mason et al 1998) found blunted GH response to

clonidine infusion in rodents that had experienced

180-min maternal separations early in life compared with

handled control rats. Taken together, the animal data are

consistent with the concept that low GH responses are

associated with early social stress, anxious behavioral

patterns, or both across several species.

Holsboer, drawing from findings in adult depression

and from animal work, has presented a model whereby the

mechanism of GH changes seen in depression may be

mediated by alterations in central corticotropin-releasing

factor (Holsboer 1995). Coplan et al (in press) reported

results consistent with this link by showing that GH

response to clonidine in the VFD monkeys was

signifi-cantly correlated with corticotropin-releasing factor in the

cerebrospinal fluid of these monkeys.

Clearly, there are several important mechanistic

ques-tions regarding the changes in GH response associated

with depression and possibly with the risk for depression

early in life. Promising preliminary results from a

nonhu-man primate model showing stable individual differences

in GH response to GHRH and clonidine in young monkeys

and a correlation between low GH response and low

behavioral reactivity in a stranger-approach condition (K.

Coleman et al, unpublished data) present one promising

approach to explore the underlying mechanisms.

Summary

Low GH response to GHRH appears to be a trait marker of

depression that is evident and stable early in development.

The mechanism and functional significance of this marker

requires further investigation. Progress in understanding

these findings will likely require both clinical and basic

investigations into the neurobehavioral systems

underpin-ning these alterations in GH regulation, including

devel-opmental and genetic influences and the possible effects of

early adverse experiences.

This work was supported by National Institute of Health Grant Nos. PO1-MH41712 (NDR and colleagues) and KO2-MH01362 (RED). This line of investigation was originally initiated under the direction of the late Joaquim Puig-Antich, to whom this article is dedicated. The authors wish to extend our appreciation to Laura Trubnick and her staff in the Sleep Laboratory, Stacy Stull and the staff of the Neuroendocrine Laboratory, the clinical interviewers, and the children and families who made this study possible.

References

(7)

Angold A, Costello EJ, Worthman CM (1998): Puberty and depression: The roles of age, pubertal status and pubertal timing.Psychol Med28:51– 61.

Birmaher B, Dahl RE, Williamson DE, Perel JM, Brent DA, Axelson DA, et al (2000): Growth hormone secretion in children and adolescents at high risk for major depressive disorder.Arch Gen Psychiatry57:867– 872.

Birmaher B, Ryan ND, Williamson DE, Brent DA, Kaufman J (1996a): Childhood and adolescent depression: A review of the past 10 years. Part II.J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry

35:1575–1583.

Birmaher B, Ryan ND, Williamson DE, Brent DA, Kaufman J, Dahl RE, et al (1996b): Childhood and adolescent depression: A review of the past 10 years. Part I. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry35:1427–1439.

Chambers WJ, Puig-Antich J, Hirsch M, Paez P, Ambrosini PJ, Tabrizi MA, Davies M (1985): The assessment of affective disorders in children and adolescents by semistructured inter-view. Test-retest reliability of the schedule for affective disorders and schizophrenia for school-age children, present episode version.Arch Gen Psychiatry42:696 –702.

Champoux MCC, Schanber SM, Kuhn CM, Suomi SJ (1989): Hormonal effects of early rearing conditions in the infant rhesus monkey.Am J Primatol19:111–117.

Charney DS, Woods SW, Goodman WK, Heninger GR (1987): Neurobiological mechanisms of panic anxiety: Biochemical and behavioral correlates of yohimbine-induced panic attacks.

Am J Psychiatry144:1030 –1036.

Coplan JD, Pine DS, Papp LA, Gorman JM (1997): A view on noradrenergic, hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis and extra-hypothalamic corticotrophin-releasing factor function in anx-iety and affective disorders: The reduced growth hormone response to clonidine.Psychopharmacol Bull33:193–204. Coplan JD, Owens MJ, Trost RC, Nemeroff CB, Smith ELP,

Rosenblum LA, et al (in press): Growth hormone response to clonidine in adversely reared primates and inhibitory role of corticotropin-releasing factor.Psychiatry Res.

Coplan JD, Trost RC, Owens MJ, Cooper TB, Gorman JM, Nemeroff CB, Rosenblum LA (1998): Cerebrospinal fluid concentrations of somatostatin and biogenic amines in grown primates reared by mothers exposed to manipulated foraging conditions.Arch Gen Psychiatry55:473– 477.

Dahl R (1996): The regulation of sleep and arousal: Develop-ment and psychopathology.Dev Psychopathol8:3–27. Dahl RE, Kaufman J, Ryan ND, Perel J, al-Shabbout M,

Birmaher B (1992): The dexamethasone suppression test in children and adolescents: A review and a controlled study.

Biol Psychiatry32:109 –26.

Dahl RE, Ryan ND (1999): Methodologic issues in sleep and neuroendocrine measures in assessment. In Shaffer D, Richters J, Christopher P, editors.Assessment in Child and Adolescent Psychopathology.New York: Guilford.

Dahl RE, Ryan ND, Birmaher B, al-Shabbout M, Williamson DE, Neidig M, et al (1991a): Electroencephalographic sleep measures in prepubertal depression.Psychiatry Res38:201– 214.

Dahl RE, Ryan ND, Matty MK, Birmaher B, al-Shabbout M, Williamson DE, Kupfer DJ (1996): Sleep onset abnormalities in depressed adolescents.Biol Psychiatry39:400 – 410. Dahl RE, Ryan ND, Perel J, Birmaher B, al-Shabbout M, Nelson

B, Puig-Antich J (1994): Cholinergic REM induction test with arecoline in depressed children.Psychiatry Res51:269 – 282.

Dahl RE, Ryan ND, Puig-Antich J, Nguyen NA, al-Shabbout M, Meyer VA, Perel J (1991b): 24-Hour cortisol measures in adolescents with major depression: A controlled study.Biol Psychiatry30:25–36.

Emslie GJ, Rush AJ, Weinberg WA, Rintelmann JW, Roffwarg HP (1994): Sleep EEG features of adolescents with major depression.Biol Psychiatry36:573–581.

Frank E, Jarrett RB, Keller MB, Martin B, Prien RF (1991): Conceptualization and rationale for consensus definitions of terms in major depressive disorder: Remission, recovery, relapse, and recurrence.Arch Gen Psychiatry48:851– 855. Gann H, Riemann D, Stoll S, Berger M, Muller WE (1995):

Growth hormone response to growth hormone-releasing hor-mone and clonidine in depression.Biol Psychiatry38:325– 329.

Garber J, Weiss B, Shanley N (1993): Cognitions, depressive symptoms, and development in adolescents.J Abnorm Psy-chol102:47–57.

Harrington R, Bredenkamp D, Groothues C, Rutter M, Fudge H, Pickles A (1994): Adult outcomes of childhood and adoles-cent depression. III. Links with suicidal behaviours.J Child Psychol Psychiatry35:1309 –1319.

Hollingshead A (1975):Four-Factor Index of Social Status.New Haven, CT: Yale University, Department of Sociology. Holsboer F (1995): Neuroendocrinology of mood disorders. In:

Bloom F, Kupfer D, editors.Neuropsychopharmacology: The Fourth Generation of Progress.New York: Raven, 957–970. Jarrett DB, Kupfer DJ, Miewald JM, Grochocinski VJ, Franz B (1994): Sleep-related growth hormone secretion is persis-tently suppressed in women with recurrent depression: A preliminary longitudinal analysis. J Psychiatr Res 28:211– 223.

Jarrett DB, Miewald JM, Kupfer DJ (1990): Recurrent depres-sion is associated with a persistent reduction in sleep-related growth hormone secretion.Arch Gen Psychiatry47:113–118. Kovacs M (1996): Presentation and course of major depressive disorder during childhood and later years of the life span.

J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry35:705–715.

Lesch KP, Laux G, Pfuller H, Erb A, Beckmann H (1987): Growth hormone (GH) response to GH-releasing hormone in depression.J Clin Endocrinol Metab65:1278 –1281. Lesch KP, Muller U, Rupprecht R, Kruse K, Schulte HM (1989):

Endocrine responses to growth hormone-releasing hormone, thyrotropin-releasing hormone and corticotropin-releasing hormone in depression.Acta Psychiatr Scand79:597– 602. Mason PTK, Plotsky P, Archer D (1998): Growth failure caused

by early maternal separation and chronic isolation.Neurosci Abstr1997:1435.

Meyer WJ III, Richards GE, Cavallo A, Holt KG, Hejazi MS, Wigg C, Rose RM. (1991): Depression and growth hormone.

J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry30:335.

Nutt DJ (1989): Altered central alpha 2-adrenoceptor sensitivity in panic disorder.Arch Gen Psychiatry46:165–169. Orvaschel H, Puig-Antich J (1987): Schedule for Affective

(8)

Pine DS, Cohen P, Brook J (1996): Emotional problems during youth as predictors of stature during early adulthood: Results from a prospective epidemiologic study.Pediatrics97:856 – 863.

Puig-Antich J, Goetz R, Davies M, Fein M, Hanlon C, Chambers WJ, et al (1984a): Growth hormone secretion in prepubertal children with major depression. II. Sleep-related plasma concentrations during a depressive episode.Arch Gen Psy-chiatry41:463– 466.

Puig-Antich J, Goetz R, Davies M, Tabrizi MA, Novacenko H, Hanlon C, et al (1984b): Growth hormone secretion in prepubertal children with major depression. IV. Sleep-related plasma concentrations in a drug-free, fully recovered clinical state.Arch Gen Psychiatry41:479 – 483.

Puig-Antich J, Kaufman J, Ryan ND, Williamson DE, Nelson B, Stull S, et al (1993): The psychosocial functioning and family environment of depressed adolescents. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry32:244 –253.

Puig-Antich J, Novacenko H, Davies M, Chambers WJ, Tabrizi MA, Karwiec V, et al (1984c): Growth hormone secretion in prepubertal children with major depression. I. Final report on response to insulin-induced hypoglycemia during a depres-sive episode.Arch Gen Psychiatry41:455– 460.

Puig-Antich J, Novacenko H, Davies M, Tabrizi MA, Ambrosini P, Goetz R, et al (1984d): Growth hormone secretion in prepubertal children with major depression. III. Response to insulin-induced hypoglycemia after recovery from a depres-sive episode and in a drug-free state.Arch Gen Psychiatry

41:471– 475.

Rao U, Ryan ND, Birmaher B, Dahl RE, Williamson DE, Kaufman J, et al (1995): Unipolar depression in adolescents: Clinical outcome in adulthood. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry34:566 –578.

Rapaport MH, Risch SC, Gillin JC, Golshan S, Janowsky DS (1989): Blunted growth hormone response to peripheral infusion of human growth hormone-releasing factor in pa-tients with panic disorder.Am J Psychiatry146:92–95.

Rosenblum LA, Coplan JD, Friedman S, Bassoff T, Gorman JM, Andrews MW (1994): Adverse early experiences affect nor-adrenergic and serotonergic functioning in adult primates.

Biol Psychiatry35:221–227.

Ryan ND, Dahl RE, Birmaher B, Williamson DE, Iyengar S, Nelson B, et al (1994): Stimulatory tests of growth hormone secretion in prepubertal major depression: Depressed versus normal children.J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 33: 824 – 833.

Ryan ND, Puig-Antich J, Ambrosini P, Rabinovich H, Robinson D, Nelson B, et al (1987): The clinical picture of major depression in children and adolescents.Arch Gen Psychiatry

44:854 – 861.

Ryan ND, Puig-Antich J, Rabinovich H, Ambrosini P, Robinson D, Nelson B, Novacenko H (1988): Growth hormone re-sponse to desmethylimipramine in depressed and suicidal adolescents.J Affect Disord15:323–337.

Sallee FR, Richman H, Sethuraman G, Dougherty D, Sine L, Altman-Hamamdzic S (1998): Clonidine challenge in child-hood anxiety disorder.J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry

37:655– 662.

Siever LJ, Trestman RL, Coccaro EF, Bernstein D, Gabriel SM, Owen K, et al (1992): The growth hormone response to clonidine in acute and remitted depressed male patients.

Neuropsychopharmacology6:165–177.

Skare SS, Dysken MW, Billington CJ (1994): A review of GHRH stimulation test in psychiatry. Biol Psychiatry 36: 249 –265.

Tancer ME, Stein MB, Black B, Uhde TW (1993): Blunted growth hormone responses to growth hormone-releasing factor and to clonidine in panic disorder.Am J Psychiatry

150:336 –337.

Gambar

Table 1. Demographic Characteristics in Depressed and Low-Risk Children
Figure 1. Growth hormone response to growth hormone–releas-ing hormone (GHRH) in children and adolescents with majorcontrol subjects (depressive disorder (F; n � 82) compared to low-risk normalE; n � 55)
Figure 3. Growth hormone response to growth hormone–releas-ing hormone (GHRH) in children and adolescents with majorcontrol subjects (depressive disorder studied during medication-free remissionfrom depression (E; n � 20) compared with low-risk normalF; n

Referensi

Dokumen terkait

Secara berkelompok, siswa diminta berdiskusi untuk menyelesaikan beberapa soal dari buku ajar dan menyajikannya di depan kelas oleh salah satu wakil kelompok..

Soromandi Tahun Anggaran 2014 pada Satuan Kerja Dinas Perhubungan Komunikasi dan Informatika Kabupaten Bima, bersama ini kami mengundang Direktur/Direktris atau yang

31 Dinas Pertanian Kota Semarang Belanja Modal Konsultan Perencana Pembangunan Instalasi Air Outlet Berpendingin. Penyediaan Sarana Produksi Pertanian/

Rp2.400.000,00 dan dibukukan pada akun biaya asuransi (dicatat sebagai biaya asuransi) maka ayat jurnal penyesuaian yang dibuat pada tanggal 31 Desember 2009 adalah ….. biaya

Konsumen akan loyal kepada perusahaan apabila konsumen telah merasa terpuaskan dan memiliki persepsi bahwa tidak ada perusahaan lain yang memiliki kualitas

Yang dilakukan oleh Sultan adalah memasang meriam-meriam, dan peluru-peluru (dari Sungsang sampai Pulau Kemaro disiapkan 60 lobang tembakan), menyiapkan rakit-rakit yang mudah

Dengan ini saya menyatakan bahwa tesis Evaluasi Mutu dan Nilai Gizi Nugget Daging Merah Ikan Tuna ( Thunnus spp) adalah karya saya sendiri di bawah bimbingan Prof.

Elastisitas harga permintaan adalah derajat kepekaan atau respon jumlah permintaan akibat berubahan harga barang atau dengan kata lain merupakan perbandingan dari