• Tidak ada hasil yang ditemukan

CONCLUSION

Dalam dokumen and Persuasion (Halaman 86-90)

Our perspective is that the studies described in this chapter open the door for new research examining the consequences of resistance to persuasion. Resistance often has been viewed as an outcome, indicating that a persuasive communi-cation has failed to affect the target attitude. Although this is sensible, resistance also has many other dimensions, as the various chapters in the current volume attest. Indeed, the research described in this chapter suggests that in some cases at least, when a persuasive attack appears to have had no impact on the target

attitude (i.e., the attack was resisted), it may actually have had a subtle yet important impact in terms of attitude certainty, attitude-relevant behavior, and the attitude’s resistance to future change. In short, resistance can be viewed as a launching pad for further theoretical and empirical exploration.

REFERENCES

Babad, E. Y., Ariav, A., Rosen, I., & Salomon, G. (1987). Perseverance of bias as a function of debriefing conditions and subjects’ confidence. Social Behaviour, 2, 185–193.

Bassili, J. N. (1996). Meta-judgmental versus operative indexes of psychological attributes: The case of measures of attitude strength. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 637–

653.

Brehm, J. W. (1966). A theory of psychological reactance. New York: Academic Press.

Brin˜ol, P., & Petty, R. E. (2003). Overt head movements and persuasion: A self-validation analysis.

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 1123–1139.

Brock, T. C. (1967). Communication discrepancy and intent to persuade as determinants of coun-terargument production. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 3, 296–309.

Cacioppo, J. T., & Petty, R. E. (1982). The need for cognition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 42, 116–131.

Crano, W. D., & Chen, X. (1998). The leniency contract and persistence of majority and minority influence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1437–1450.

Fazio, R. H., & Zanna, M. P. (1978). Attitudinal qualities relating to the strength of the attitude-behavior relationship. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 14, 398–408.

Festinger, L. (1950). Informal social communication. Psychological Review, 57, 271–282.

Festinger, L. (1954). A theory of social comparison processes. Human Relations, 7, 117–140.

Festinger, L. (1957). A theory of cognitive dissonance. Evanston, IL: Row, Peterson.

Fishbein, M., & Ajzen, I. (1975). Belief, attitude, intention, and behavior. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.

Gilbert, D. T. (1998). Ordinary Personology. In D. Gilbert, S. Fiske, & G. Lindzey (Eds.), Handbook of social psychology (Vol. 2, pp. 89–150). Boston: McGraw-Hill.

Greenwald, A. G. (1968). Cognitive learning, cognitive response to persuasion, and attitude change.

In A. G. Greenwald, T. C. Brock, & T. M. Ostrom (Eds.), Psychological foundations of attitudes (pp. 147–170). New York: Academic Press.

Gross, S., Holtz, R., & Miller, N. (1995). Attitude certainty. In R. E. Petty & J. A. Krosnick (Eds.), Attitude strength: Antecedents and consequences (pp. 215–245). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

Heider, F. (1958). The psychology of interpersonal relations. New York: Wiley.

Hovland, C. I., Janis, I. L., & Kelley, H. H. (1953). Communication and persuasion: Psychological studies of opinion change. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.

Jacks, J. Z., & Devine, P. G. (2000). Attitude importance, forewarning of message content, and resistance to persuasion. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 22, 19–29.

Jacoby, L. L., & Kelley, C. M. (1987). Unconscious influences of memory for a prior event. Per-sonality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 13, 314–336.

Killeya, L. A., & Johnson, B. T. (1998). Experimental induction of biased systematic processing:

The directed-thought technique. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 24, 17–33.

Krosnick, J. A., & Abelson, R. P. (1992). The case for measuring attitude strength in surveys. In J.

Tanur (Ed.), Questions about questions (pp. 177–203). New York: Russell Sage.

Lewan, P. C., & Scotland, E. (1961). The effects of prior information on susceptibility to an emo-tional appeal. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 62, 450–453.

Lydon, J., Zanna, M. P., & Ross, M. (1988). Bolstering attitudes by autobiographical recall: Attitude persistence and selective memory. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 14, 78–86.

McGuire, W. J. (1964). Inducing resistance to persuasion: Some contemporary approaches. In L.

Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 1, pp. 191–229). New York:

Academic Press.

McGuire, W. J. (1968). Personality and attitude change: An information-processing theory. In A. G.

Greenwald, T. C. Brock, & T. M. Ostrom (Eds.), Psychological foundations of attitudes (pp. 171–196). New York: Academic Press.

Miller, N. E. (1965). Involvement and dogmatism as inhibitors of attitude change. Journal of Ex-perimental Social Psychology, 1, 121–132.

Petty, R. E., Brin˜ol, P., & Tormala, Z. L. (2002). Thought confidence as a determinant of persuasion:

The self-validation hypothesis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82, 722–741.

Petty, R. E., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1986). Communication and persuasion: Central and peripheral routes to attitude change. New York: Springer-Verlag.

Petty, R. E., & Krosnick, J. A. (Eds.). (1995). Attitude strength: Antecedents and consequences.

Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

Petty, R. E., Ostrom, T. M., & Brock, T. C. (Eds.) (1981): Cognitive responses in persuasion. Hills-dale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Petty, R. E., Tormala, Z., & Rucker, D. D. (in press). Resistance to persuasion: An attitude strength perspective. In J. T. Jost, M. R. Banaji, & D. Prentice (Eds.), Perspectivism in social psychology:

The yin and yang of scientific progress. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

Petty, R. E., & Wegener, D. T. (1999). The Elaboration Likelihood Model: Current status and con-troversies. In S. Chaiken & Y. Trope (Eds.), Dual-process theories in social psychology (pp. 41–

72). New York: Guilford.

Rokeach, M. (1960). The open and closed mind. New York: Basic Books.

Rucker, D. D., & Petty, R. E. (in press). When resistance is futile: Consequences of failed counter-arguing for attitude certainty. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology.

Schwarz, N., Bless, H., Strack, F., Klumpp, G., Rittenauer-Schatka, H., & Simons, A. (1991). Ease of retrieval as information: Another look at the availability heuristic. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 195–202.

Strack, F., & Fo¨rster, J. (1998). Self-reflection and recognition: The role of metacognitive knowledge in the attribution of recollective experience. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 2, 111–

123.

Swann, W. B., Pelham, B. W., & Chidester, T. R. (1988). Change through paradox: Using self-verification to alter beliefs. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 268–273.

Tannenbaum, P. H., Macauley, J. R., & Norris, E. L. (1966). Principle of congruity and reduction of persuasion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 3, 233–238.

Tormala, Z. L., & Petty, R. E. (2002). What doesn’t kill me makes me stronger: The effects of resisting persuasion on attitude certainty. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 1298–1313.

Tormala, Z. L., & Petty, R. E. (2003). Resisting persuasion by the skin of one’s teeth: The hidden success of failed persuasive communications. Unpublished manuscript. The Ohio State Univer-sity.

Tormala, Z. L., Petty, R. E., & Brin˜ol, P. (2002). Ease of retrieval effects in persuasion: A self-validation analysis. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28, 1700–1712.

Wood, W., Lundgren, S., Ouellette, J. A., Busceme, S., & Blackstone, T. (1994). Minority influence:

A meta-analytic review of social influence processes. Psychological Bulletin, 115, 323–345.

Wu, C., & Shaffer, D. R. (1987). Susceptibility to persuasive appeals as a function of source cred-ibility and prior experience with the attitude object. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-ogy, 52, 677–688.

Zuwerink, J. R., & Devine, P. G. (1996). Attitude importance and resistance to persuasion: It’s not just the thought that counts. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 931–944.

83

5

Individual Differences in Resistance to Persuasion:

The Role of Beliefs and Meta-Beliefs

Pablo Brin˜ol

Universidad Auto ´ noma de Madrid

Derek D. Rucker

The Ohio State University

Zakary L. Tormala

Indiana University

Richard E. Petty

The Ohio State University

Within the persuasion literature, the most common treatment of the term “resis-tance” involves the outcome of showing little or no change to a persuasive message, (e.g., McGuire, 1964). However, to understand resistance truly, one must recognize that the study of resistance is more than simply the outcome of a persuasive message. Petty, Tormala, and Rucker (in press; see also Tormala

& Petty, this volume) noted recently that within the persuasion literature, resis-tance has also been examined as a psychological process (e.g., one can resist by counterarguing), a motivation (i.e., having the goal of not being persuaded), and a quality of an attitude or person (i.e., being resistant to persuasion).

Many of the chapters in the present volume discuss variables or situations that affect the outcome of resistance (e.g., Jacks & O’Brien, this volume; Tormala &

Petty, this volume; Quinn & Wood, this volume), the process of resistance (e.g., Wegener, Petty, Smoak, & Fabrigar, this volume), or people’s motivation to resist (e.g., Knowles & Linn, this volume). The present chapter differs in that our inter-est lies in identifying personality attributes or aspects of the individual related to resistance. We focus our discussion on individual differences in resistance ex-pected to be constant across topics, sources, situations, and so on. Thus, we deal primarily with resistance as a quality of a person. In this chapter, we introduce a new meta-cognitive approach to understand individual differences in resistance.

That is, we examine individuals’ meta-cognitions about their own resistance. We begin by providing a brief review of past work on individual differences in resis-tance, and we suggest that future research could benefit from adopting a meta-cognitive perspective. Finally, we present empirical evidence and apply the meta-cognitive framework to understand: how individuals’ beliefs affect their actual resistance to persuasion (resistance as an outcome); the qualities an dividual possesses (resistance as a quality); and the processes by which an in-dividual chooses to resist (resistance as a process) when his or her goal is to resist (resistance as a motivation).

Dalam dokumen and Persuasion (Halaman 86-90)