• Tidak ada hasil yang ditemukan

General and specific thinking skills and

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2017

Membagikan "General and specific thinking skills and"

Copied!
12
0
0

Teks penuh

(1)

ContentslistsavailableatScienceDirect

Thinking

Skills

and

Creativity

jo u r n al hom e p ag e : htt p : / / w w w . e l s e v i e r . c o m / l o c a t e / t s c

General

and

specific

thinking

skills

and

schooling:

Preparing

the

mind

to

new

learning

Mari-Pauliina

Vainikainen

,

Jarkko

Hautamäki,

Risto

Hotulainen,

Sirkku

Kupiainen

UniversityofHelsinki,DepartmentofTeacherEducation,CentreforEducationalAssessment,Finland

a

r

t

i

c

l

e

i

n

f

o

Articlehistory:

Received3October2014

Receivedinrevisedform17April2015 Accepted23April2015

Availableonlinexxx

Keywords:

Thinkingskills Cognitivedevelopment Formalthinking

Class-effectonthinkingskills Nestedmultilevelfactorialmodels

a

b

s

t

r

a

c

t

Enhancingthinkingskillsisanimportantgoalofformaleducation.Itisoftenembedded innationalcurricula,which,however,areseldombasedontheoreticalunderstandingof thestructureoftheskillsorhowtheyshouldbetaught.Accordingly,thereisonlylimited informationavailableaboutschools’successinthisimportanttask.Thepresentstudyhas twogoals:firstly,tofindsupportforthetheoreticalassumptionofthenestedstructureof thinkingskillswithacorefactorofformalthinkingandspecialisedstructuresforverbaland quantitativereasoning;andsecondly,totestthedifferentiateddevelopmentoftheseskills inschool.Thiswasdonebystudyingclass-levelvariationofsixthgraders’thinkingskillsin amultilevelfactoranalysiswheninitialbetween-classdifferencesatgradethreehadbeen takenintoaccount.Thedata(N≈1543)weredrawnfromalearningtolearnpanelstudyin oneofthemajorcitiesofFinland.Theresultsshowedthatthecorefactorforformalthinking couldbeidentifiedatboththeindividualandtheclasslevel,andthatattheindividuallevel therewerestatisticallysignificantresidualfactorsforverbalandquantitativereasoning. Initialbetween-classdifferencesexplainedonlyathirdofthevarianceofclass-levelformal thinking.Thiswasinterpretedtoindicatetheeffectofschooling.

©2015PublishedbyElsevierLtd.

1. Generalandspecificthinkingskillsandschooling

Discussionsofcurrentworkingenvironmentsandoftheskillsnecessaryforworkcallfora newapproach towards learning.Inthecontinuouslychangingworkenvironmentspeopleneedtomakecoherentdecisionswithaccessto unlim-itedinformationinalimitedtime,tothinkcreatively,toadjusttheiractionsandattitudesaccordingtopossiblerisksand problems,tolearnquickly,and totrusttheirproblemsolvingskills(Halpern,2008).Hence,educationalpolicymakers world-widehavelatelybecomeinterestedinconcepts,suchaslearningtolearn,thinkingskills,and21stcenturyskills (Recommendation2006/962/ECoftheEuropeanParliamentandoftheCouncil,2006;Rocardetal.,2007;Organisationfor EconomicCo-operationandDevelopment(OECD),2013a).

Thecommoncoreforthenewornewlyintroducedconceptsisthattheyalltapunderlyingcognitivecompetencesand non-curriculardomain-generalskillsthatregardanindividual’soveralllearningpreparedness.Dependingontheframework, theseskillsarereferred toascross-curricular, learningtolearn(LTL), transversal,or21stcenturyskills(DeakinCrick, Stringer,&Ren,2014).Regardlessoftheterm,thesedefinitionsemphasisetheimportanceofdevelopingthinkingskillsas

Correspondingauthorat:CentreforEducationalAssessment,UniversityofHelsinki,P.O.Box9,00014Helsinki,Finland.Tel.:+358503465050.

E-mailaddress:mari-pauliina.vainikainen@helsinki.fi(M.-P.Vainikainen). http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tsc.2015.04.006

(2)

abasisforfuturelearningfromcoreprocessesandspecialisedstructuralsystemstocriticalthinking(Demetriou,2014). Enhancingthinkingskillshavebeenconsideredasacentralgoalofeducationalreadyfordecades(Resnick,1987),butit isstillquiterarethateducationalsystemspromotesuchdevelopmentdeliberately(aninterestingexceptionisHungary, wherenewframeworksinreading,mathematicsandscienceareallbasedonideasofgeneralandspecificthinkingasan explicitpart,seeCsapó&Szapo,2014).Eventhoughthinkingskillsareexpectedtoimprovewhenpupilsproceedthrough theirformalschooling,strategiesforevaluatingeducationaloutcomesandeffectivenessofschoolingmainlycentreonthe measurementofsubject-specificknowledgeandskillsoratbesttheirapplication.InFinland,theassessmentofgeneral cognitivecompetencesandtheaffectivefactorswhichsupporttheireffectiveuse,togetherlabelledLTL,weredefinedasone ofthemeasurableoutcomesofeducationalreadyinthe1990s(Hautamäkietal.,2002;NationalBoardofEducation,1999). Sincethen,theyhavebeenusedasoneindicatorformonitoringtheeffectivenessofeducation.

LTLisdefinedasthecognitivecompetenceandwillingnesstoadopttonoveltasksandnewlearning(Hautamäkietal., 2002;Hautamäki,Hautamäki,&Kupiainen,2010).InempiricalassessmentofLTL,theassessmenttasksareseentoactivate acomplexofinterrelatedcompetencesandbeliefs,leadingtoanattempttosolvethetasks.Competencereferstothe applicationofgeneralcognitiveschemasandthealreadyacquiredknowledgeorscholasticachievementtonewsituations. Beliefsrefertoanticipatedemotionswhich,onceactivated,leadtocommitmentorrefusal.Definedthisway,LTLcompetences arerelatedtointelligence,understoodinaPiagetianframeworkastheactiveuseofformaloperationalschemas.Fromthis followsthehypothesisthatthecognitiveLTLtasksmeasuregeneralthinkingskills(seeAdey&Csapó,2014).LTLasan educationalgoalisanexplicitpartoftheEUdefinitionofkeycompetencesandofthe21stcenturyskillsintheglobalcontext. However,neitherofthetwopresentsanempiricallytestedmodeltomeasurethecompetences.TheFinnishLTLFramework andscalesareoneoffewattemptstoofferatooltoassessboththecognitiveandthewillingness-orcommitment-related componentsofLTL(DeakinCricketal.,2014).

Theaimofthepresentstudyisontheonehandtotestthetheoreticalassumptionthatthethinkingskillsmeasuredwith theFinnishLTLconstructhaveanestedstructure(cf.,Härnqvist,Gustafsson,Muthén,&Nelson,1994)withformaloperational thinking(seeShayer,1979)atitscoreandwithspecialisedresidualfactorsforverbalproportionalandquantitativereasoning (seeDemetriou,Spanoudis,&Mouyi,2011).Ontheotherhand,theaimistoevaluatewhetherschoolinghasaneffecton theseskillsintheFinnishcontext,wherethinkingskillsaredefinedasagoalembeddedinallschoolsubjectsinthenational corecurriculum(NationalBoardofEducation,2004)butdetailsregardingtheirteachingaremissing.

1.1. Developmentofthinking

Developmentalpsychologistshavelongstudiedthedevelopmentofthinking.Thetheoryofcognitivedevelopment pro-posedbyDemetriouetal.(2011)andDemetriou(2014)involvesbothcentralandgeneralmechanisms,andspecialised capacitysystemsfordifferentdomainsofknowledgeorrelations.Thespatial,verbal,quantitative,categorical,causal,and socialreasoningsystemshavebeenidentifiedbymethodsfromdifferenttheoreticalorigins,andtheyareconsideredas autonomousdomainsofunderstanding,thinking,andproblemsolving.Acriticalfeatureofthistheoryisthatthe develop-mentofthespecialisedsystemsisbothlimitedbyandistherouteintothedevelopmentofthegeneralintellectualprocessor anditsexecutivecontrol(self-regulation).Thatis,thegeneralfactorisalsoamenabletoeducationalinfluence.Thereis evi-dencefromothertheoreticalbackgroundsthathighperformanceonagenerallevelfacilitatestheacquisitionofnewdomain specificskillsespeciallyontheearlyphasesofthelearningprocess(Francis,Fletcher,Maxwell,&Satz,1989;Gustafsson, 2008).Butwhenlearningisbasedonalreadyacquiredskills,thegainsaremorelikelytodependonearlierdomain-specific knowledge.Accordingly,theimprovementsarelikelytobedomain-specific.Allthismeansthatgoodsubject-specific teach-ingmakestheconnectionbetweenspecificknowledge,e.g.theuseofspecificconcepts,withthegeneraluseofconcepts, rulesandtheirapplication.Thisgraduallyleadstogainsalsointhefunctioningofthegeneralmechanisms(Demetriouetal., 2011;Gustafsson,2008).

Oneofthemoststudiedconstructsinthedevelopmentofgeneralthinkingskillsandthefunctioningofthegeneral mechanisms(cf.,Demetriouetal.,2011)isthecontrolofvariablesstrategy(CV),whichisalsooftenreferredtoasthe vary-one-thing-at-a-time(VOTAT) strategy.ItwasfirstintroducedbyInhelderandPiaget(1958)aspartoftheformal operationalthinkingconstruct(seeShayer,1979).RegardlessofcriticismsconcerningexplicitnessofInhelderandPiaget’s workitstillprovidesoverarchingillustrationhowadolescents’cognitivecompetencesdevelopduringtheseconddecadeof theirlife(Kuhn,2008).Theemergenceofformaloperationsataroundage12–15involvesreasoningbasedonhypotheses, independentofconcreteobjects,whichmeans“therealissubordinatedtotherealmofthepossible”(Piaget,2006).Age variationisseentobeingrainedinthedifferingintellectualstimuliinchildren’senvironmentsandtodependonpersonal interestsandexperiences.However,formalthinkingisnotnecessarilyappliedallthetimeoracrossalldomains(Piaget,1972, 2006).Evenifindividualdifferencesinformaloperationalthinkingarerelatedtointelligence,verbalabilityandexecutive functions,theseareconsideredtobepartlyculturallybound(Emick&Welsh,2005).Ithasbeenshownthatthatthecontrol ofvariablesstrategyiscentraltoscienceandanessentialskillattainableandtrainablebythetimechildrenarecognitively advancingfromaconcretetowardaformaloperationallevel(Neimark,1975;Shayer,2008).

(3)

Fernandes,2007;Rohde&Thomson,2007;Sternberg,1999).Generalcognitionisexpressedorinvestedinvariousways indifferentlearningsituationsandschoolachievements(Gustafsson&Carlstedt,2006).Butthedevelopmentallyoriented interpretationofgeneralcognitionasanexpressionofgrowthoflogicalthinkingopensupanoptionforintervention(Adey, Csapó,Demetriou,Hautamäki,&Shayer,2007;Shayer,2008).Thisintegratedunderstanding,educatingthedevelopingmind, iswellrepresentedintheworkofDemetriou(2014)andDemetriouetal.(2011).

AccordingtoDemetriou’stheory,weexpectthethinkingskillsmeasuredwithintheFinnishLTLcontexttobestructured inawaythatinadditiontothecoreofformalthinking,residualfactorsofallthespecialisedreasoningsystemsaretobe found.Inpractice,onlytheverbalandquantitativefactorscanbeidentifiedhereasthesixthgradeversionoftheFinnishLTL scaleusedinthepresentstudydoesnothavesufficientnumberofitemsontheareasofspatial,causalorsocialreasoning. Eventhoughninthgradedatawithalargerscopeofcognitivetaskswouldhavebeenavailable,12-year-oldswereselected asthetargetgroupofthepresentstudyduetothesensitivenessofthisagegroupinregardtoboththedevelopmentof formalthinking(Piaget,2006),andtotheenhancementofthinkingskillsingeneral(Csapó,1997).

1.2. Enhancingthedevelopmentofthinkingskills

Manyresearchersseethatinductionprocesses,suchasperformingmentalcomparisonsformabasisfor knowledge-building.This,inturn,hasbeenshowntohaveatransfereffectonlearninginschoolsubjects(Adey&Shayer,1994;Adhami &Yates2008;Bloom,Engelhart,Furst,Hill,&Krathwohl,1956; Klauer&Phye,2008;Tomic&Klauer,1996).Thus, by promotingthedevelopmentofthinkingskills,e.g.howtoputthingsinorder,classifyorperformmentalrotations,we simultaneouslyenhancefundamentallearningskills,whichhavetransfereffectsacrosstheschoolcurriculum.Thisalso facilitatestheattainmentofhigherorderthinkingskills(Anderson&Krathwohl,2001;Shayer,2008).

Itcouldbearguedthattherearetwomainwaystopromotethinkingskillsinschool.Thefirstapproachcanbecalled explicitwithspecificeducationalprogrammes,whichareexpectedtopromotethinkingskillsiftheprogrammeguidelines areappropriatelyfollowed.Theseprogrammescanbefurtherdividedintotwocategoriesaccordingtohowthetraining ofthinkingskillsisarranged(McGuinness&Nisbett,1991).Thefirstcategory,thecontent-basedmethod,consistsof pro-grammeswhichensurethatthinkingskillsaretaughtwithinconventionalcurriculumareas(e.g.mathematicsorscience). Awell-studiedprogrammerepresentingthismethodistheCognitiveAcceleration(CA)Programme(Adey&Shayer,1994) basedonPiagetiancognitivestages.Programmesbelongingtothesecondcategorysharetheideathatthinkingskillscan betaughtassuch(e.g.Feuerstein,Rand,Hoffman,&Miller,1980;Klauer,1989;Klauer,1991).Thepromotionofgeneric skillshasbeencriticisedfordevelopingthinkingskillsa-contextuallyandinhibitingthetransferofthedevelopedskillsto newcontexts.Yet,programmesbasedonKlauer’stheoryhaverecentlyshownstrongevidencefortransfereffectonboth fluidintelligenceandvariousacademicsubjects(Klauer&Phye,2008;Molnár,2011;Molnár,Greiff,&Csapó,2013). Regard-lessofstrongevidencebynowthatspecificprogramscanpromote21stcenturyskills,theirlarge-scaleusehasnotbeen widelyspreadtotheeducationalfieldduetobothshort-sightededucationalpolicy-decisionmakingandlimitedresources ofstakeholders(Pellegrino&Hilton,2012).

Thesecondapproachcanbedescribedasanimplicitone.Inthisapproach,curriculadonotperseemphasisesystematic cultivationofthinkingskillsbutsuchdevelopmentisexpectedtoresultasaside-effect(Kuhn,2005;Molnár,2011).Especially intheNordiccountries,approaches,practices,andinitiativestowardsenhancingthinkingskillsareverylimited.Forexample, intheFinnishnationalcorecurriculum,thinkingskillsareregardedtobelongintoeachconventionalcurriculumarea.Thus, itisexpectedthattheimplementedcurriculumwithannuallymoredemandingcontentcomplexity(spiralcurriculum; cf.Bruner,1960)raisesimplicitlythelevelofthinkingskills,aswell.However,researchhasshownthatthepromotion ofthinkingskillsneedstobeeitherintegratedpurposefullyinthecurriculumortheyhavetobeexplicitlytaughttogain effectiveresults(Adey&Shayer,1994;Kuhn,2005;Nisbett,1993).

AsinFinlandexplicitmethodsforteachinggeneralthinkingskillsarenotpointedoutinthecorecurriculumandthesame classteachertypicallyteachestheirclassfromthirdtosixthgrade,theFinnishschoolsystemallowsustostudyteacher-and class-relatedvariationinthedevelopmentofgeneralthinkingskills.Theexpectedclass-levelvariationinthedevelopment ofthinkingskillscouldprovideevidenceforthatevenimplicitteachingofthinkingskillscanbebeneficial.Itcanalsogive estimatesabouthowmuchoralternativelyhowlittleteacherscaninfluencethedevelopinggeneralthinkingskills.

1.3. Educationalassessmentandthinkingskills

(4)

foundbetween-classdifferencescallforfurtherresearchtogainamorecomprehensivepictureregardingthedevelopment of21stcenturyskillsinrelationtosubject-specificlearningandtore-evaluatetheequityoftheFinnisheducationsystem (cf.,Sahlberg,2007).Inthepresentstudy,afirststeptowardsthisdirectionistakenbyanalysingclass-levelvariationinthe thinkingskillsmeasuredbytheFinnishLTLscale.Thelongitudinaldataofthepresentstudyallowscontrollingforinitial differencesbetweenclasses,revealingsomeofthepossibleeffectsschoolinghasproducedascomparedtocross-sectional studies,whichmightjustindicatethenon-randomallocationofpupilsinclasses(cf.,Vainikainen,2014).

1.4. Researchquestionsandhypotheses

Asdescribedabove,thestructureofthinkingskills,theirdevelopmentineducationalsettings,andeducationalequity haveallbeenextensivelystudiedindependentfromeachother.However,thereisverylittleresearch,whichcombinesthese perspectivesandevaluatestowhatextentthinkingskillsreallydevelopasaresultofformalschoolingiftheyarenottaught explicitly.Therefore,theresearchquestionsandthehypothesesofthepresentstudyare

Q1:DothethinkingskillsmeasuredbytheFinnishLTLscaleshaveanestedhigher-orderstructureasthetheoriesabove suggest?

H1:Thereisanestedhigher-orderstructure:thehigher-ordergeneralthinkingskillsfactorisdeterminedbyformal thinkingbutthereareseparablefactorsforverbalandquantitativereasoningskills(Demetriouetal.,2011;Gustafsson, 2008;Piaget,2006;Shayer,1979).

Q2:Istherebetween-classvariationinformalthinkingandtheresidualfactorsofverbalandquantitativereasoning? H2:Thereisbetween-classvariationinallthesefactors,anditexplainsbetween10%and20%ofthevariance(Hautamäki etal.,2013;YangHansenetal.,2014).

Q3:Isthebetween-classvariationexplainedbyinitialdifferencesbetweenclasses,whichhasinearlierstudiesbeen showntoexplainabout5%ofthevariancealreadyatthestartingpoint(Vainikainen,2014),ordoesittellaboutdifferent trajectoriesofdevelopmentindifferentclasses?Thatis,havethedifferencesattheendofsixthgradebeenpartiallycaused byschoolingasthinkingskillscanbeemphasiseddifferentlyinteachingwhenexplicitmethodsarenotspecifiedinthe Finnishcurriculum?

H3:Theclass-levelvariationinthinkingskillsdoesnotonlyreflectinitialdifferencesbetweenclasses:ithasbeenshown earlierthattowardstheendofprimaryschool,between-classdifferencesexplain10–20%ofthevarianceofperformanceon differentdomains,whichismuchmorethaninitialdifferencesbetweenclasses(Vainikainen,2014YangHansenetal.,2014). Toourknowledge,therearenopriormultilevellongitudinalstudiesexplainingthepossibleschoolingeffectsonthinking skills.

2. Methods

ThedataweredrawnfromtheVantaapanelstudy,inwhichfourfullagecohorts(thefirst,third,sixthandninthgraders) ofthemunicipalitywerefollowedfrom2010to2013.Thepresentstudyutilisesthesixthgradedatafrom2013which, regardingthemeasuresusedherewerefirstreportedbyKrkovic,Greiff,Kupiainen,Vainikainen,andHautamäki(2014) andVainikainen(2014).Noneofthesestudieswasaboutthenestedstructureofthinkingskills.Morerecently,weused thenestedmodelinanotherarticleacceptedforpublicationinaFinnishspecialeducationjournal(Vainikainen,Hienonen, HautamäkiandHotulainen,inpress).Inthearticle,weappliedonlyindividual-levelmodellingandshowedthatclasssize didnotexplainthedevelopmentofformalthinkingofpupilswithspecialeducationneeds.Regardingthenestedmodel,we referredtothecurrentstudyastheoriginalpublicationanddidnotrepeatanyoftheresultspresentedhere.

2.1. Participants

Thestudiedcohortoriginallyconsistedof2113sixthgradersin118classesin37schools.Duetothepre-definedexclusion criteria,8smallspecialeducationclassesdidnotparticipate,andabout5%ofpupilswereabsentduetosicknessorother reasons.20%ofthepupilswererandomlyassignedtoapaper-basedassessmentgroupwhereas80%completedthe computer-basedversion(CBA)oftheLTLtest.OnlythedataoftheCBAgroup(N=1543,49.2%girls)wereusedinthepresentstudy, andthefinalnumberofclasseswithsufficientdataformultilevelmodellingwas102.TheageofthepupilswasM=12.67, SD=.43.Longitudinaldatacomprisingalsopupils’thirdgraderesultsinanalogicalreasoning,whichwasutilisedfortesting thelasthypothesis,wereavailablefor1303pupils.ParentswereinformedthroughtheCityEducationDepartment,and77% oftheparentsalsofilledoutabackgroundquestionnaire,whichisnotreportedhere.

2.2. Measures

(5)

Table1

Reliabilitiesofthecognitivetasksandtheattitudescales.

Scale Number of items ˛

Verbalproportionalreasoning 10 .55

Quantitativereasoning 15 .75

Formalthinking 4 .62

Analogicalreasoningthirdgrade 8 .78

sometimesfoundgoldandsilvertogether,sometimeshehasfoundsilverbyitself,everyothertimehehasfoundneither silvernorgold.Whichofthefollowingruleshasbeentruefortheprospector?Goldandsilverarefoundtogether,never apart.;ifhefoundsilverthenhefoundgoldwithit.;ifhefoundgoldthenhefoundsilverwithit.;ifhefoundgoldthenhe didnotfindsilver;seeBond,2010formoredetails).Inthelatter,thepupilsweregivenonepremiseandtheconclusion(e.g. Conclusion:LakeSaimaaistoocoldforswimming.Firstfact:thetemperatureofLakeSaimaais5centigrades),andtheyhad tochoosefromamongstfivealternativesthesecondpremisewhichwouldmaketheconclusionvalid(e.g.Mostlakesaretoo coldforswimming.;itiswintertime.;fivedegreewateristoocoldforswimming.;LakeSaimaaisalwayscold.;swimming incoldwaterisnofun.).Theitemswerescoreddichotomouslyascorrectorincorrect.

QuantitativereasoningwasassessedbysevenitemsadaptedfromtheHiddenArithmeticalOperatorstaskbyDemetriou, Platsidou,Efklides,Metallidou,andShayer(1991)andeightitemsbasedontheInventedMathematicalConceptstaskof Sternberg,Castejon,Prieto,Hautamäki,&Grigorenko(2001).Inthefirstsevenitemstherewereonetofourhiddenoperators (e.g.Whichoperatordotheaandbstandforin(5a3)b4=6?).Inthelatteritems,twoinventedarithmeticaloperatorswere conditionallydefined(e.g.ifa>b,lagstandsforsubtraction,butifa≤b,itstandsformultiplication).Afterthedefinition,the pupilshadtoselectthecorrectanswerfromamongstfourmultiplechoicealternativestoeightitems(e.g.Howmuchis7 lag5?).Theitemswerecodeddichotomouslyforthewholeequation.

FormalthinkingwasmeasuredbytheControlofVariablestask,whichisamodifiedversion(Hautamäki,1984seealso Hotulainen,Thuneberg,Hautamäki,&Vainikainen,2014,foradetaileddescriptionofthetask)ofShayer’sScienceReasoning Task‘Pendulum’(1979).ItisbasedononeoftheformalschemataidentifiedbyInhelderandPiaget(1958).Thepupilswere presentedwithfouritemsintheformofcomparisonssetintheworldofFormula1raceswithfourvariables:driver,car, tires,andtrack,withtwoalternativesforeach.Thepupilshadtojudgewhetherthesingleeffectofthedriver,car,tires,and trackcouldbeconcludedfromthecomparison(e.g.Round1:driverRäikkönen,carFerrari,tiresMichelin,trackHockenheim; Round2:driverHamilton,carFerrari,tiresMichelin,trackMonaco;canyouconcludetheeffectsofdriver/car/tires/track ontheresult?)Thereweretwocomparisonswith3or4Yes/No-choicesforthevariablesandtwocomparison-setstobe complemented.Thefouritemswerecodeddichotomouslyforacorrectanswertoallofthevariablesintheitem.

Fortestingthelasthypothesis,pupils’analogicalreasoningscoresfromthreeyearsearlierweremergedintheotherwise cross-sectionaldata.ThetaskwasadaptedfromthegeometricanalogiestestofHosenfeld,vandenBoom,andResing(1997). Thepupilswerepresentedapairofgeometricfigures,thatis,asmallsquareontheleftandabigsquareontheright.The taskwastoapplythesamerulewhenchoosingapairfromfiveoptionsforanotherfigure,thatis,asmallcircle,between5 answeroptions.Thetransformationsincludedaddinganelement,changingsizesandpositions,halvinganddoubling.The maximumnumberofsimultaneoustransformationswasfive.Theeightitemswerecodeddichotomously,andthenumber ofcorrectitemswasconvertedtopercentage.

ThereliabilitiesofthemeasuresarepresentedinTable1.Eventhoughsomeofthereliabilitiesweresomewhatlowdue tothesmallnumberofitems,thereliabilitieswereconcludedacceptableforpopulation-levelstudies.

2.3. Statisticalanalyses

DescriptivestatisticswerecalculatedwithSPSS22.Single-andmulti-levelnestedfactorialmodelsweretestedinMplus 7.2(Muthén&Muthén,2012)usingtheMLRestimatorwithoutimputationofmissingvalues.Toreducethenumberof parameterstobeestimated,theitemsofthesubtestswereparcelledintofourverbalreasoning,fourquantitativereasoning, andtwoformalthinkingparcels,whichweretreatedascontinuousvariables.AccordingtotherecommendationofMarsh, Lüdtke,Nagenkast,Morin,andVonDavier(2013),weusedahomogeneousparcellingstrategy,inwhichitemsfromdifferent theoreticaloriginswerenotmixedinthesameparcelsandtheitemsinoneparcelwereassimilaraspossible.Before parcelling,wecalculatedIRTitemdiscriminationestimatesforthethreescales,ensuringthesufficientunidimensionality ofthescales(seeMarshetal.,2013).ThemodelswereconsideredashavingagoodfitwithCFI&TLI>.95andRMSEA<.05, andanacceptablefitwithCFI&TLI>.90andRMSEA<.08.Alsoreportedare2values,butduetothelargesamplesizeand thenumberofparameterstobeestimatedsignificantp-valuesweretobeexpected.Accordingly,modelswithsignificant p-valueswerenotdiscardedifalltheotherindiceswereacceptable.

3. Results

3.1. Descriptivestatistics

(6)

Table2

Descriptivestatisticsofthevariablesusedinmodelling.

Variable

Parcel N Min Max M SD ICC

Verbal proportional 1 1522 .00 1.00 .53 .30 .07

Verbalproportional2 1512 .00 1.00 .54 .39 .07

Verbalproportional3 1506 .00 1.00 .51 .30 .05

Verbalproportional4 1504 .00 1.00 .23 .32 .02

Quantitative1 1501 .00 1.00 .52 .28 .10

Quantitative2 1401 .00 1.00 .10 .22 .03

Quantitative3 1475 .00 1.00 .57 .29 .08

Quantitative4 1471 .00 1.00 .35 .28 .07

Formal1 1495 .00 1.00 .59 .45 .07

Formal2 1480 .00 1.00 .37 .44 .07

Analogical reasoning 1303 0.00 100.00 40.19 29.88

N = Number of responses, Min = minimum value, Max = maximum value, M= Mean, SD = Standard deviation, ICC = Intraclass correlation.

Fig.1.Theexpectednestedstructureofthinkingskills.Theerrortermsofthefactorindicatorsarenotpresentedinthefigure.

3.2. Hypothesis1

Inthefirsthypothesisweexpectedthatthemeasuredthinkingskillswouldhaveanestedfactorialstructure.Thatis,the higher-ordergeneralthinkingskillsfactorwouldbedeterminedbyPiagetianformalthinkingbuttherewouldnevertheless beseparablefactorsforverbalproportionalandquantitativereasoningskills.Totestthishypothesis,themodelpresented inFig.1wasfirsttestedonindividual-leveldatawithoutmodellingbetween-levelvariance.

Thenested modelofFig.1fittedthedatawell(CFI=.978,TLI=.962,RMSEA=.034,2=73.411,df=26,p<.001)and showedabetterfitthananalternativemodelwiththreenon-nestedcorrelatedfactors(CFI=.954,TLI=.935,RMSEA=.045, 2=131.972,df=32,p<.001)oramodelwithoutcorrelationsbetweenthesecondaryfactorsofverbalproportionaland quantitativereasoning.ThestandardisedfactorloadingsforthenestedmodelarepresentedinTable3.

(7)

Table3

Standardisedfactorloadingsintheindividual-levelmodel.

Parcel Formal Verbal proportional Quantitative

Verbalproportional1 .48*** .12*

Verbalproportional2 .46*** .39***

Verbalproportional3 .36*** .33***

Verbalproportional4 .24*** .27***

Quantitative1 .45*** .21***

Quantitative2 .31*** .17***

Quantitative3 .35*** .54***

Quantitative4 .42*** .57***

Formal1 .59***

Formal2 .73***

*p<.05. ***p<.001.

slightlyhigherfactorloadingsandstatisticallysignificantownvariance(s2=0.004,p<.010).Theseresultstogetherindicate thatquantitativereasoningskillswereclearlyseparablefromthemoregeneralformalthinkingskills,whereasthesame wasnottrueforthemeasuredverbalthinkingskills.Thus,theresultsgaveonlypartialsupporttothefirsthypothesis.

3.3. Hypothesis2

Inthenexthypothesisweassumedthattherewouldbebetween-classeffectsthatwouldexplain15–20%ofthevariance bothinformalthinkingandinthemorespecificverbalproportionalandquantitativereasoningskills.Thebetween-level factorswerespecifiedforformalthinkingcontainingallparcelsandforquantitativereasoning.Abetween-levelverbal proportionalreasoningfactorcausedproblemswiththefitindicesandhadtoberemoved.Themodelfitthedatawell (CFI=.968,TLI=.949,RMSEA=.030,2=135.548,df=57,p<.001).Unlikeinthesingle-levelmodelofH1,thequantitative reasoningfactorwasnotstatisticallysignificantattheclasslevelandinthismodelitdidnothavestatisticallysignificant ownvarianceatanylevelregardlessofitsstatisticallysignificantfactorloadingsattheindividuallevel.Thus,theformal thinkingfactorcapturedallpupil-levelandclass-levelvariation(ICC=.18)inthismodel.

Addingtheclasslevelinthemodelchangedslightlythefactorloadingsattheindividuallevelcomparedtothesingle-level model.Thewithinandbetween-levelfactorloadingswithoutanyconstraintsacrossthetwolevelsarereportedinTable4. Table4showsthatattheclasslevel,alltheparcelsexceptthelastparcelforverbalproportionalreasoninghadstrong loadingsontheformalthinkingfactor.ItwasconcludedthatH2waspartiallysupportedregardingtheclass-leveleffectson formalthinking.

3.4. Hypothesis3

Inthelasthypothesisitwasassumedthattheclass-levelvariationinthinkingskillswouldnotonlyreflectinitial dif-ferencesbetweenclassesbutwouldbepartiallycausedbyschooling.Totestthishypothesis,longitudinaldataintheform ofanalogicalreasoningtestscoresfromthethirdgradewereused,whichwereavailableforabout85%ofthepupils.Third gradeanalogicalreasoningskillswereaddedinthetwo-levelmodelspecifiedinH2asbothwithin-leveland between-levelpredictorsofformalthinking.Themodelfitwasacceptablealsoforthismodel(CFI=.958,TLI=.939,RMSEA=.034,

Table4

Standardisedfactorloadingsinthetwo-levelmodel.

Within-level Between-level

Parcel Formal Verbalproportional Quantitative Formal Quantitative

Verbal proportional 1 .45*** .11* .84***

Verbalproportional2 .42*** .39*** .98***

Verbalproportional3 .32*** .33*** .92***

Verbalproportional4 .27*** .30*** .58(ns.)

Quantitative1 .40*** .17** .86*** .56*

Quantitative2 .30*** .16** .71*** .41(ns.)

Quantitative3 .32*** .52*** .70*** .48(ns.)

Quantitative4 .40*** .60*** .79*** .42(ns.)

Formal1 .55*** .97***

Formal2 .71*** .94***

(8)

Fig.2.Thefinaltwo-levelmodel.Theparcelnamesandtheerrortermsarenotpresentedinthefigure.***p<.001,ns.=non-significant.

2=185.772,df=75,p<.001).Asexpected,thirdgradeanalogicalreasoningskillspredictedsixthgradeformalthinkingat theindividuallevel(ˇ=.57,SE=.03,p<.001)andattheclass-level(ˇ=.58,SE=.14,p<.001),explaining32%and34%ofits variance,respectively.Astheverbalproportionalandquantitativefactorsturnedouttobenon-significantinthetwo-level model,itwasalsotestedwhethertheycouldbecompletelyomittedfromthemodel.However,thisdecreasedthepredictive powerofthirdgradeanalogicalreasoningskills,indicatingthatthesenon-significantfactorsneverthelessremovedsome irrelevantvariancefromtheformalthinkingfactor.Thefinaltwo-levelmodelispresentedinFig.2.

Asonlyathirdofthebetween-levelvarianceofformalthinkingdemonstratedinthesixthgradecouldbeexplainedby initialdifferencesbetweentheclasseswhenmeasuredbytheanalogicalreasoningtest,itwasconcludedthattherewastrue varianceleftaftercontrollingforinitialdifferencesandthatschoolingduringtheinterimyearscanhaveproducedaneffect onformalthinking.Thus,H3wassupported.

4. Discussion

Enhancingthinkingskillscanbeseenasoneofthemanytasksofschooling(Olson,2003),anditcanbearguedthatin the21stcenturythistaskhasbecomeincreasinglyimportantduetothechangesinworkinglife(DeakinCricketal.,2014). InFinland,thinkingskillsareembeddedinthecore-curriculumforbasiceducationbutthereisonlylimitedinformation availableabouthowschoolssucceedinthistask.Moreover,ithasnotbeenstudiedinanylargerscale,whethertheteaching ofthinkingskillsislimitedtosubject-specificareasorifschoolingmanagestotargetthemoregeneralthinkingskillsaswell. Therefore,thepresentstudyhadtwogoals:ontheonehand,theaimwastofindsupportforthetheoreticalassumption derivingfromPiaget(2001)andShayer(2008),andmorerecentlyfromDemetriouetal.(2011),aboutthinkingskillshaving anestedstructurewithitscoreinlogicalorformalthinkingbutwithadditionalspecialisedstructures.Ontheotherhand, theeducabilityoftheseskillsinacontextwherethinkingskillsareembeddedinthecurriculumwastestedbystudying class-levelvariationinformalthinkingatgradesixwheninitialdifferencesbetweenclasseshadbeentakenintoaccount. ThedataweredrawnfromalearningtolearnpanelstudyinoneofthemajorcitiesinFinland,whereafullcohort(N≈2000) ofpupilswereassessedatgradethreeandsixbytheFinnishLTLscale(Hautamäkietal.,2002).Thecognitivesubtasksof thescalecoveredformalthinking,verbalproportionalreasoning,andquantitativereasoning,whichcanallbeconsidered critical21stcenturyskills(Adey&Csapó,2014).

(9)

Shayer(1979,2008)andDemetriou(2014),whoclaimthattherearethecoreoperators,whichareneededineverytask.The resultsalsofittogetherwiththefindingsofHärnqvistetal.(1994),eventhoughtheirterminologydifferssomewhatfrom theconceptsusedinthisstudy.Also,theresidualverbalfactorwasintheirstudyofagreaterimportance,mostlikelydue tothenatureofthetasksused.

Asthefirstresultssupportedthehigher-orderstructureofthemeasuredskills,weproceededtotesttheclass-level variationofboththegeneralformalandthequantitativethinkingskills.Somewhatunexpectedly,inthetwo-levelmodel, thevarianceofthequantitativefactorwasnolongerstatisticallysignificantattheindividuallevel,inadditiontonothaving muchrelevanceattheclasslevel.However,asitclearlyremovedsomeoftheirrelevantvarianceintheformalthinking factor,itwaskeptinthemodel.TheresultsshowedthatjustasinthestudyofHärnqvistetal.(1994),therewasclass-level variationthataccountedfor18%ofthevarianceinthegeneralfactorofformalthinking.Thetestingofthethirdhypothesis showedthatthisvariationwasonlypartiallyexplainedbyinitialbetween-classdifferencesasmeasuredbyananalogical reasoningtestatgradethree.Asearlieranalogicalreasoningskillsareindicatorsofthefunctioningofthesamegeneral mechanismsthatatthelaterstageofdevelopmentcanbemeasuredbyformalthinkingtasks(Demetriouetal.,2011),itwas concludedthatpupils’formalthinkingskillshadclearlydevelopeddifferentlyindifferentclasses.Eventhoughtheremaybe otherfactorsexplainingthedevelopment,whichaccountedforapproximately12%ofthetotalvariance,forinstance socio-economicbackgroundandselectiveclasses(Härnqvistetal.,1994),theresultssupportthepossibilitythatformalthinking skillscanhavebeenenhanceddifferentlybydifferentteachers.Here,itisimportanttorememberthatanyspecifictraining programmesforthinkingskillswerenotapplied.Thus,themaincontributionofthisstudyisnotthatthehigher-orderfactor structurewasfound,butthatthegeneralfactor–hereformalthinking–couldbegivenaninterpretation,whichcanbetied toeducationalinterventions,evenifjustembeddedineverydayteaching.Alsotheresidualfactors,eveniftheirindependent variancesweredifferentatindividualandclass-levels,openwindowsforteaching-relatedinterventions.

4.1. Limitationsofthestudy

Thedatasetsomelimitstothepossibleresearchquestionsandtheanalysestobeperformed.Themultilevelanalyses couldbeconductedonlyattheclass-leveleventhoughitwouldhavebeeninterestingtolookatbetween-schooleffectsas well–especiallywhenthedifferencesbetweenschoolshavetraditionallybeenverysmallinFinlandalsointhelearning tolearnassessments(seeHautamäkietal.,2013).This,however,wasnotpossiblewiththepresentdataduetothesmall numberofschools.Therefore,itwouldbeimportanttoreplicatethisstudywithasampleconsistingofsufficientnumberof schoolsforschool-levelMFA.

Therearealsolimitationsregardingthemeasures:Theverbaltasksmeasuredproportionallogicalthinkinganddeductive reasoningbutdidnotcoverthefullscopeofverbalreasoningskills.Thismaybethereasonforthenon-significantvariance oftheverbalproportionalfactor.Regardingthelongitudinaldata,theinitialthirdgradeinitialdifferencesweremeasured onlybytheanalogicalreasoningtask,whichonlyaccountsforsomeofthepossiblesourcesofinitialvariationbetweenthe classes.Ifothermeasureshadbeenusedinadditiontoanalogicalreasoning,theshareofunexplainedvarianceofsixthgrade formalthinkingmighthavebeensomewhatsmaller.However,astheunexplainedsharewasashighastwothirdsofthe variance,theresultspointstronglytothedirectionthatthereistruevariationproducedbyclasses.Theremainingclass-level variancewasnotrelatedtopupils’initialcognitivecompetencethatoftenexplainsaboutathirdofthevariance(cf.,Duncan etal.,2007),justasinthepresentstudy,ortheveryusualnon-randomallocationofpupilsinclassesinFinnishschools(see Vainikainen,2014).

4.2. Conclusionsandpracticalimplications

(10)

reasoning,andseparationandcontrolofvariables.Accordingly,theLTLframeworkcanbeusedtoguidetheadvancementof thinkingskills,beitthroughinterventionortheirconsciousimbeddinginthecurriculum,andthesuccessofthisadvancement canbeassessedusingtheLTLscale,describedinthispaper.Bycombiningthistotheassessmentofsubject-specificoutcomes willallowfurtheranalysingtheactualorpotentialroleofthevarioussubjectsinitsadvancement(seeAdey&Shayer,2002; Titcombe,2015).

Pupilsneedtobepreparedtoseenewoptionsinsteadofobstacles,butasChristinaHowe(Howe,McWilliam,&Cross, 2005)expresses:“Chancefavoursonlythepreparedmind.”Thewell-preparedmindis,atleast,a personwitha good commandofformal,logicalandscientificthinking,andawillingnesstoapplythatskillforsolvingopenproblems.However, asKuhnandHolman(2011,p.65)remark:“...mostoftheempiricalevidencerelatedtoadolescentcognitiveskillsdoes notpresentapictureofstrongandsecurecompetence,makingthequestionofhowtosupportcognitivedevelopment inadolescenceapressingone”.Theyalsoexpresstheneedtoextendresearchoncognitivedevelopmentfrom control-of-variablesstrategystudiestostudiesregardingmultiplecausalityandevidence-basedpredictionstoreachthelevelof measuringstudents’readinesstoentertheworldofintellectualdiscoursetoparticipateincommunitiesofintellectual discourseAlsotheFinnishLTLassessmentislackinginthisrespect.YetthefindingsofthestudyshowthatLTLcanbeused tomeasurethedevelopmentofprimaryschoolpupils’thinkingskillstoasufficientdegreetoevaluatethesuccessoftheir fosteringatschool

AppendixA.

Descriptivesoftheoriginaldichotomousvariables.

N Min Max M SD

Verbal1 1515 0 1 0.69 0.46

Verbal2 1507 0 1 0.35 0.48

Verbal3 1504 0 1 0.55 0.5

Verbal4 1485 0 1 0.43 0.5

Verbal5 1500 0 1 0.64 0.48

Verbal6 1495 0 1 0.6 0.5

Verbal7 1487 0 1 0.69 0.46

Verbal8 1472 0 1 0.24 0.43

Verbal9 1467 0 1 0.27 0.44

Verbal10 1496 0 1 0.19 0.39

Quantitative1 1500 0 1 0.86 0.34

Quantitative2 1438 0 1 0.64 0.48

Quantitative3 1401 0 1 0.38 0.46

Quantitative4 1398 0 1 0.12 0.32

Quantitative5 1376 0 1 0.18 0.38

Quantitative6 1287 0 1 0.08 0.27

Quantitative7 1278 0 1 0.03 0.17

Quantitative8 1468 0 1 0.72 0.45

Quantitative9 1453 0 1 0.66 0.47

Quantitative10 1426 0 1 0.51 0.5

Quantitative11 1449 0 1 0.38 0.49

Quantitative12 1440 0 1 0.31 0.46

Quantitative13 1454 0 1 0.28 0.45

Quantitative14 1401 0 1 0.19 0.4

Quantitative15 1452 0 1 0.62 0.49

Formal1 1491 0 1 0.58 0.49

Formal2 1474 0 1 0.61 0.49

Formal3 1467 0 1 0.39 0.49

Formal4 1379 0 1 0.37 0.48

N=Numberofresponses,Min=minimumvalue,Max=maximumvalue,M=Mean,SD=Standarddeviation.

AppendixB.

Correlationmatrix.

Variable B C D E F G H I J K

A)Verbalproportional1 .308** .284** .273** .225** .240** .180** .101** .243** .312** .230** B)Verbalproportional2 .172** .228** .147** .143** .140** .097** .176** .203** .191**

C)Verbalproportional3 .460** .126** .177** .195** .119** .219** .273** .254**

D)Verbalproportional4 .220** .266** .218** .169** .230** .316** .338**

E)Quantitative1 .276** .213** .146** .294** .335** .255**

F)Quantitative2 .296** .223** .261** .337** .305**

G)Quantitative3 .164** .203** .261** .251**

H)Quantitative4 .126** .184** .177**

I)Formal1 .430** .287**

(11)

References

Adey,P.,&Csapó,B.(2014).Developingandassessingscientificreasoning.InB.Csapó,&G.Szabo(Eds.),Frameworkfordiagnosticassessmentofscience. BudapestNemzetiTankönyvkiadó.

Adey,P.,Csapó,B.,Demetriou,A.,Hautamäki,J.,&Shayer,M.(2007).Canwebeintelligentaboutintelligence?Whyeducationneedstheconceptofplastic generalability.EducationalResearchReview,2,75–97.http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.edurev.2007.05.001

Adey,P.,&Shayer,M.(1994).Reallyraisingstandards.InCognitiveinterventionandacademicachievement.London:Routledge. Adey,P.,&Shayer,M.(2002).Learningintelligence.OpenUniversityPress.

Adhami,M.,&Yates,C.(2008).Thinkingandcurriculum.InP.Adey(Ed.),Let’sthinkhandbook–aguidetocognitiveaccelerationintheprimaryschool(pp. 57–70).London:GLAssessment.

Anderson,L.W.,&Krathwohl,D.R.(Eds.).(2001).Ataxonomyforlearning,teaching,andassessing:ArevisionofBloom’staxonomyofeducationalobjectives. NewYork:Longman.

Bloom,B.S.,Engelhart,M.D.,Furst,E.J.,Hill,W.H.,&Krathwohl,D.R.(1956).Taxonomyofeducationalobjectives:Theclassificationofeducationalgoals. InHandbookI:Cognitivedomain.NewYork:DavidMcKayCompany.

Bond,T.G.(1995).BLOT:Bond’sLogicalOperationsTest.Townsville,Australia:JamesCookUniversity[originalworkpublishedin1976]. Bond,T.G.(2010).Comparingdécalageanddevelopmentwithcognitivedevelopmentaltests.JournalofAppliedMeasurement,11(2),158–171. Bruner,J.(1960).Theprocessofeducation.Cambridge,Mass:HarvardUniversityPress.

Carroll,J.B.(1989).TheCarrollmodel:A25-yearretrospectiveandprospectiveview.EducationalResearcher,18(1),26–31.

Csapó,B.(1997).Developmentofinductivereasoning:Cross-sectionalmeasurementsinaneducationalcontext.InternationalJournalofBehavioural Development,20(4),609–626.

Csapó,B.,&Szapo,G.(2014).Frameworkfordiagnosticassessmentofscience.BudapestNemzetiTankönyvkiadó.

DeakinCrick,R.,Stringher,C.,&Ren,K.(Eds.).(2014).Learningtolearn.Internationalperspectivesfromtheoryandpractice.London:Routledge. Deary,I.J.,Strand,S.,Smith,P.,&Fernandes,C.(2007).Intelligenceandeducationalachievement.Intelligence,35,13–21.

Demetriou,A.(2014).Learningtolearn,knowandreason.InR.DeakinCrick,C.Stringher,&K.Ren(Eds.),Learningtolearn.Internationalperspectivesfrom theoryandpractice.London:Routledge.

Demetriou,A.,Platsidou,M.,Efklides,A.,Metallidou,Y.,&Shayer,M.(1991).Thedevelopmentofquantitative-relationalabilitiesfromchildhoodto adolescence:Structure,scaling,andindividualdifferences.LearningandInstruction,1,19–43.

Demetriou,A.,Spanoudis,G.,&Mouyi,A.(2011).Educatingthedevelopingmind:Towardsanoverarchingparadigm.EducationalPsychologyReview,23(4), 601–663.http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10648-011-9178-3

Duncan,G.J.,Dowsett,C.J.,Claessens,A.,Magnuson,K.,Huston,A.C.,Klebanov,P.,etal.(2007).Schoolreadinessandlaterachievement.Developmental Psychology,43(6),1428–1446.http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.43.6.1428

Emick,J.,&Welsh,M.(2005).AssociationbetweenformaloperationalthoughtandexecutivefunctionasmeasuredbythetowerofHanoi-revised. LearningandIndividualDifferences,15(3),177–188.

Feuerstein,R.,Rand,Y.,Hoffman,M.,&Miller,M.(1980).Instrumentalenrichment:Aninterventionprogrammeforcognitivemodifiability.Baltimore,MD: UniversityParkPress.

Francis,D.J.,Fletcher,J.M.,Maxwell,S.E.,&Satz,P.(1989).Astructuralmodelfordevelopmentalchangesinthedeterminantsofreadingachievement. JournalofClinicalChildPsychology,18(1),44–51.

Gustafsson,J.-E.(2002).Measurementfromahierarchicalpointofview.InH.I.Braun,D.H.Jackson,&D.E.Wiley(Eds.),Theroleofconstructsin psychologicalandeducationalmeasurement.Mahwah:Erlbaum.

Gustafsson,J.-E.(2008).Schoolingandintelligence:Effectsoftrackofstudyonlevelandprofileofcognitiveabilities.InP.E.Kyllonen,R.D.Roberts,&L. Stankov(Eds.),Extendingintelligence.Enhancementandnewconstructs(pp.31–50).NewYork:Routledge.

Gustafsson,J.-E.,&Carlstedt,B.(2006).Abilitiesandgradesaspredictorsofachievement:Theencapsulationtheory.InPaperpresentedatthesymposium, theinvestmenttheoryofintelligence:Newevidence,newchallengesgivenattheannualmeetingoftheAmericanPsychologicalAssociationNewOrleans, August10–13,2006.

Halpern,D.F.(2008).Isintelligencecriticalthinking?Whyweneedanewdefinitionofintelligence.InP.C.Kyllonen,R.D.Roberts,&L.Stankov(Eds.), Extendingintelligence.Enhancementandnewconstructs(pp.293–310).NewYork:Routledge.

Hautamäki,J.(1984).Peruskoululaistenloogisenajattelunmittaamisestajaesiintymisestä.[Measuringandtheoccurrenceoflogicalthinkingamongbasic schoolstudents].Joensuunyliopistonyhteiskuntatieteellisiäjulkaisuja1.Joensuu:JoensuunYliopistopaino.

Hautamäki,A.,Hautamäki,J.,&Kupiainen,S.(2010).Assessmentinschools–learningtolearn.InP.Peterson,E.Baker,&B.McGaw(Eds.),International encyclopediaofeducation(Vol.3)(pp.268–272).Oxford:Elsevier.

Hautamäki,J.,Arinen,P.,Eronen,S.,Hautamäki,A.,Kupiainen,S.,Lindblom,B.,etal.(2002).Assessinglearning-to-learn:Aframework.Helsinki:National BoardofEducation.Evaluation4/2002.

Hautamäki,J.,&Kupiainen,S.(2014).LearningtolearninFinland.Theoryandpolicy,researchandpractice.InR.DeakinCrick,C.Stringher,&K.Ren(Eds.), Learningtolearn.Internationalperspectivesfromtheoryandpractice.Routledge.

Hautamäki,J.,Kupiainen,S.,Marjanen,J.,Vainikainen,M.-P.,&Hotulainen,R.(2013).Oppimaanoppiminenperuskoulunpäättövaiheessa:Tilannevuonna 2012jamuutosvuodesta2001[Learningtolearnattheendofbasiceducation:Thesituationin2012andthechangefrom2001].UniversityofHelsinki, DepartmentofTeacherEducationResearchReports347.Helsinki:Unigrafia.

Higgins,S.,Hall,E.,Baumfield,V.,&Mosely,D.(2005).Ameta-analysisoftheimpactofimplementationofthinkingskillsapproachesonpupils.In Researchevidenceineducationlibrary.London:EPPI-Centre,SocialScienceResearchunit,InstituteofEducation,UniversityofLondon. Hosenfeld,B.,vandenBoom,D.C.,&Resing,W.C.M.(1997).Constructinggeometricanalogiestestforthelongitudinaltestingofelementaryschool

children.JournalofEducationalMeasurement,34(4),367–372.

Hotulainen,R.,Thuneberg,H.,Hautamäki,J.,&Vainikainen,M.-P.(2014).Measuredattentioninprolongedover-learnedresponsetasksanditscorrelation tohighlevelscientificreasoningandschoolachievement.PsychologicalTestandAssessmentModeling,56(3),237–254.

Howe,C.,McWilliam,D.,&Cross,G.(2005).Chancefavoursonlythepreparedmind:Incubationandthedelayedeffectsofpeercollaboration.British JournalofPsychology,96,67–93.

Härnqvist,K.,Gustafsson,J.-E.,Muthén,B.O.,&Nelson,G.(1994).Hierarchicalmodelsofabilityatindividualandclasslevels.Intelligence,18,165–187. Inhelder,B.,&Piaget,J.(1958).Theearlygrowthoflogicinthechild.London:Routledge&KeganPaul.

Klauer,K.J.(1989).DenktrainingfürkinderI.Göttningen:Hogrefe. Klauer,K.J.(1991).DenktrainingfürkinderII.Göttningen:Hogrefe.

Klauer,K.J.,&Phye,G.D.(2008).Inductivereasoning:Atrainingapproach.ReviewofEducationalResearch,78(1),85–123. Kuhn,D.(2005).Educationforthinking.Cambridge:HawardUniversityPress.

Kuhn,D.(2008).Formaloperationsfromatwenty-firstcenturyperspective.HumanDevelopment,51(1),48–55.

Kuhn,D.,&Holman,A.(2011).Whatarethecognitiveskillsadolescentsneedforlifeinthetwenty-firstcentury?InE.Amsel,&J.G.Smetana(Eds.), Adolescentvulnerabilitiesandopportunities.Developmentalandconstructivistperspectives.Cambridge:CambridgeUniversityPress.

Kupiainen,S.,Hautamäki,J.,&Rantanen,P.(2008).EUpre-pilotonlearningtolearn:Reportonthecompileddata.2008-1190/001-001TRA-TRINDC. Kuusela,J.(2000).Tieteellisenparadigmanmukaisenajattelunkehittyminenperuskoulussa[Thedevelopmentofscientificthinkinginbasiceducation].

UniversityofHelsinki,DepartmentofTeacherEducation,ResearchReports.

(12)

Marsh,H.W.,Lüdtke,O.,Nagenkast,B.,Morin,A.J.S.,&VonDavier,M.(2013).Whyitemparcelsare(almost)neverappropriate:Twowrongsdonotmake aright–camouflagingmisspecificationwithitemparcelsinCFAmodels.PsychologicalMethods,18(3),257–284.http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0032773 McGuinness,C.,&Nisbett,J.(1991).TeachingthinkinginEurope.BritishJournalofEducationalPsychology,61(2),171–186.

Molnár,G.(2011).Playfulfosteringof6-to8-year-oldstudents’inductivereasoning.ThinkingSkillsandCreativity,6(2),91–99. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tsc.2011.05.002

Molnár,G.,Greiff,S.,&Csapó,B.(2013).Inductivereasoning,domainspecificandcomplexproblemsolving:Relationsanddevelopment.ThinkingSkills andCreativity,9(8),35–45.

Muthén,L.K.,&Muthén,B.O.(2012).Mplususer’sguideversion7.

NationalBoardofEducation.(1999).AframeworkforevaluatingeducationaloutcomesinFinland.NationalBoardofEducation.Evaluation8/1999. NationalBoardofEducation.(2004).Nationalcorecurriculumforbasiceducation2004.

http://www.oph.fi/english/publications/2009/national core curricula for basic education

Neimark,E.(1975).Intellectualdevelopmentduringadolescence.InF.Horowitz(Ed.),Reviewofchilddevelopmentresearch(pp.541–594).Chicago UniversityPress:Chicago.

Nisbett,J.(1993).Thethinkingcurriculum.EducationalPsychology,13(3–4),281–290.

OECD.(2013a).PISA2012assessmentandanalyticalframework:Mathematics,reading,science,problemsolvingandfinancialliteracy.OECDPublishing. http://dx.doi.org/10.1787/9789264190511-en

OECD.(2013b).PISA2012results:Whatstudentsknowandcando?Studentperformanceinmathematics,readingandscience(volumeI).OECDPublishing. http://dx.doi.org/10.1787/9789264201118-en

OECD.(2014).PISA2012results:Creativeproblemsolving:Students’skillsintacklingreal-lifeproblems(volumeV).OECDPublishing. http://dx.doi.org/10.1787/9789264208070-en

Olson,D.R.(2003).Psychologicaltheoryandeducationalreform:Howschoolremakesmindandsociety.CambridgeUniversityPress.

Pellegrino,J.W.,&Hilton,M.L.(Eds.).(2012).Educationforlifeandwork:developingtransferableknowledgeandskillsinthe21stcentury.Washington,DC: TheNationalAcademiesPress.NationalResearchCouncil.CommitteeonDefiningDeeperLearningand21stCenturySkills,BoardonTestingand AssessmentandBoardonScienceEducation,DivisionofBehavioralandSocialSciencesandEducation.

Piaget,J.(1972).Psychologyandepistemology:Towardsatheoryofknowledge.Harmondsworth:Penguin. Piaget,J.(2001).Studiesinreflectingabstraction.Hove:PsychologyPress.

Piaget,J.(2006).Reason.NewIdeasinPsychology,24(1),1–29.

Recommendation2006/962/ECoftheEuropeanParliamentandoftheCouncilof18December2006onkeycompetencesforlifelonglearning.Official JournalL394of30.12.2006.

Resnick,L.(1987).Educationandlearningtothink.Washington.DC:NationalAcademyPress.

Rocard,M.,Csermely,P.,Jorde,D.,Lenzen,D.,Walberg-Henriksson,H.,&Hemmo,V.(2007).Scienceeducationnow:Arenewedpedagogyforthefutureof Europe.EU.Read29.09.2012.<http://ec.europa.eu/research/science-society/document library/pdf 06/report-rocard-on-science-education en.pdf> Rohde,T.E.,&Thomson,L.A.(2007).Predictingacademicachievementwithcognitiveability.Intelligence,35,83–92.

Ross,J.D.,&Ross,C.M.(1979).Rosstestofhighercognitiveprocesses.Novato,California:AcademicTherapyPublications.

Sahlberg,P.(2007).Educationpoliciesforraisingstudentlearning:TheFinnishapproach.JournalofEducationPolicy,22(2),147–171. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02680930601158919

Shayer,M.(1979).HasPiaget’sconstructofformaloperationalthinkinganyutility?BritishJournalofEducationalPsychology,49,265–276. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8279.1979.tb02425.x

Shayer,M.(2008).Intelligenceforeducation:AsdescribedbyPiagetandmeasuredbypsychometrics.BritishJournalofEducatiomalPsychology,78,1–29. Sternberg,R.(1999).Intelligenceasdevelopingexpertise.ContemporaryEducationalPsychology,24(1),359–375.

Sternberg,R.,Castejon,J.L.,Prieto,M.D.,Hautamäki,J.,&Grigorenko,E.(2001).ConfirmatoryfactoranalysisoftheSternbergtriarchicabilitiestestin threeinternationalsamples.EuropeanJournalofPsychologicalAssessment,17,1–16.

Thuneberg,H.,Hautamäki,J.,Hotulainen,R.,2015.Scientificreasoning,SchoolachievementandGender:AMultilevelStudyofbetweenandwithinschool effectsinFinland.ScandinavianJournalofEducationalResearch01/2015.

Titcombe,R.(2015).Learningmatters.Thetruthaboutourschools(Kindleed.).Paperback.

Tomic,W.,&Klauer,K.J.(1996).Ontheeffectsoftraininginductivereasoning:Howfardoesittransferandhowlongdotheeffectspersist?European JournalofPsychologyofEducation,11(3),283–299.

Vainikainen,M.-P.(2014).Finnishprimaryschoolpupils’performanceinlearningtolearnassessments:Alongitudinalperspectiveoneducationalequity. Unigrafia:UniversityofHelsinki,DepartmentofTeacherEducationResearchReports,360.Helsinki.

Vainikainen,M.-P.,Hienonen,N.,Hautamäki,J.,&Hotulainen,R.Tukeatarvitsevienoppilaidenajattelutaitojenkehittyminenerikokoisissaluokissa[The effectofclasssizeonthedevelopmentofthinkingskillsofstudentswithsupportneeds].NMIBulletin.[inpress].

Gambar

Table 1
Fig. 1. The expected nested structure of thinking skills. The error terms of the factor indicators are not presented in the figure.
Table 4
Fig. 2. The final two-level model. The parcel names and the error terms are not presented in the figure

Referensi

Dokumen terkait

[r]

1.699.000.000,- (Satu Milyard enam ratus sembilan Puluh sembilan juta rupiah). Demikian untuk diketahui dan atas perhatianya disampaikan

Setelah dilakukan pembuktian kualifikasi terhadap data dan informasi yang tercantum dalam dokumen penawaran, sama dengan dokumen asli yang ada pada dokumen penawaran

Upaya yang dilakukan oleh Alisha Fancy Shop dalam meningkatkan serta mempertahankan hubungan dengan pelanggan jangka panjang untuk memperoleh sebuah loyalitas

Oleh karena itu, dapat dilakukan penelitian lebih lanjut tentang penentuan lokasi yang sesusai untuk budidaya rumput laut di Kota Batam ini agar masyarakat di

Banyuasin Tahun Anggaran 2014, berdasarkan Berita Acara Hasil Pengadaan Langsung Nomor :. 170/PL/Nakertrans/APBD/Penlatker/2014 tanggal 23 Oktober 2014, dan Surat

Populasi yang penulis tentukan untuk kepentingan penelitian ini adalah Mahasiswa PKK Program Studi Pendidikan Tata Boga Angkatan 2010 yang sudah menyelesaikan mata

Ekstensifikasi pada jenis tanah regosol memberikan peluang yang besar karena tanah ini memiliki konsistensi yang menguntungkan untuk pertumbuhan umbi bawang merah, namun tanah