• Tidak ada hasil yang ditemukan

Nonexpertise Negative Space or Negation (1)

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2018

Membagikan "Nonexpertise Negative Space or Negation (1)"

Copied!
17
0
0

Teks penuh

(1)

draft version--do not cite without author's permission

This paper resulted from pulling at one seemingly minor thread in my own research and discovering just how many other areas of scholarship it was connected to. When I initially set out to study the interactions among what I framed as knowledge experts in central Mexico, I had little hint that those whom I was by default categorizing as nonexperts would become

theoretically important in their own right. Once I dove into the literature on expertise, it became apparent that this nonexpert category was used unproblematically yet posed significant

theoretical, ethical, and practical challenges. As Fuller and Collier (2004:236) note, "an ironic consequence of the increasing division of cognitive labor in society is that more of us, for more of the time, share the role of nonexpert. This universal sense of nonexpertise is the epistemic basis for reconstructing the public sphere today". If this is so, why does nonexpertise go

undertheorized? What would happen if studies of expertise took nonexpertise equally seriously? To complement the other papers on this panel, and to perhaps spur further investigation, I will focus on nonexpertise. For context, I begin with a brief description of how the issue arose in the course of my fieldwork. The paper continues with a selective overview of the conventional scholarship treating nonexpertise. And in the last section I present a critical appraisal of two approaches to rethinking nonexpertise already in evidence in the scholarship.

Knowledge politics and nonexpertise under the volcano

(2)

Since the volcano's 1994 reactivation, potentially threatening millions of Mexicans in the capital and four contiguous states, the federal government has employed academic earth scientists to generate sensor data and computer models that would serve as the basis for disaster

management efforts (Macías Medrano 2005). These volcanologists and geophysicists, whether placing or checking sensors, attending public meetings in what they defined as the high-risk zone, or giving interviews to broadcast news, are consistently brought into proximity with

representatives of the mountain towns' residents. Prominent among them are the latest in a long line of indigenous ritual practitioners, known variously astiemperos,graniceros,quiatlazquis, andmisioneros del temporal. The purview of these "weather-workers" includes interpreting meteorological and volcanic phenomena, organizing offerings to the volcano spirit, don

Gregorio, and his companion in nearby Iztaccíhuatl, Rosita, and healing certain weather-linked illnesses (Albores and Broda 1997; Broda, Iwaniszewski, and Montero 2001; Glockner 1996). The other counterbalance to the scientific and technocratic views of the volcano scientists and their state patrons comes from domestic UFO investigators,ovniólogos(from OVNI, objeto volador no identificado) orufólogos(Martínez Jiménez 1992; Sheaffer 1998) who collect reports of aerial anomalies in the vicinity of the volcano. Engaged with these experts is a range of interested participants: government functionaries; media organizations and journalists; anthropologists; and local residents.

(3)

broadens its social relevance but loses its unique enlightening and reality-defining powers (Loudin 2003; Pfadenhauer 2006), drew the bulk of my attention at the time. Competitors seeking expert status singled out one figure to use as a counterexpert foil in dealing with those thereby designated as nonexperts: for instance, a number of volcanologists expressed

exasperation with ufologists for adding to the media-consuming public's confusion on what was and was not "real" science (Cook 2004:232 233).

While I did not elaborate on the nonexpert role in my dissertation beyond a brief

speculative aside, it has loomed increasingly large in my thinking. For instance, the strategy of volcano residents is in many cases to perform their nonexpertise for the experts in the

government and the academy, repeatedly professing ignorance of official data and plans, yet in so doing they gain some measure of control over how disaster management plans are

implemented. Media personnel and government functionaries play the role of nonexperts as interlocutors with the scientific experts on the public s behalf, both to communicate official plans and to reinforce scientific expertise and authority. The disclaimer of nonexpertise is even uttered by other scholars, possessing doctorates and university positions and public roles, when they defer to their volcano science peers, usually in the restricted matters of geophysics or monitoring and modeling.

Ground against figure: nonexpertise in recent scholarship

(4)

"novice", "amateur", and "everyday" all appear, sometimes woven into elaborate typologies, sometimes used indiscriminately, yet rarely clearly or independently defined.

Nonexpertise is integral to this ever-expanding scholarship, yet in a manner analogous to negative space in art. It is a relative absence surrounding the subject that makes it stand out all the more clearly. Indeed, nonexpertise, when it is treated at all, is usually defined by negation: nonexperts are whatever experts are not, or they lack whatever experts have. The negativity falls into several key categories, which I will summarize below, preserving in so far as possible the divergences and disagreements in the literature.

Nonexpertise and who knows

The basis for defining and delineating (non)expertise in much of the expertise literature tends to be epistemic. Experts have more knowledge than nonexperts, or they have special knowledge that the latter do not, and this imbalance constitutes the rationale for their autonomy and power (Ericsson 2006; Margolis 1996). Such a quantitative view has characterized such various efforts as constructing so-called "expert systems" in artificial intelligence (Buchanan, R. Davis, and Feigenbaum 2006) and the multifarious programs to improve the "public understanding of science" (Irwin and Wynne 1996) and the consequent compliance of nonexperts-as-objects.

(5)

al. 2007), or isolating deception (Reinhard et al. 2002). Yet in most other studies, significant, measurable differences are invoked to set the two apart.

A third differentiation, mainly in studies of sports or other performances, is that nonexperts manifest some other deficit of ability, though a similar dispute regarding the potential acquisition of this ability appears in the literature. These include rate of improvement and ultimate level of attainment in motor tasks (Howard 2009), perceptual acuity and accuracy (Cousin and Siegrist 2010; Mann et al. 2007), recall from memory (André and Fernand 2008), and problem solving (Ayton 1992; Sánchez-Manzanares, Rico, and Gil 2008).

Nonexpertise and who matters

A second major way in which nonexperts figure in the literature, if in an occluded way (Fuller and Collier 2004:236), is the moral and practical matter of whose input counts in decision-making, especially in exceptional situations like crises. The essential question is: who merits deference on technical issues? Increasing reliance on experts causes complications for firms--in which changes to customary decision-making are subverted by specialization (Mohr 1994)--and for liberal democracies--in which rough political equality is challenged by epistemic inequality (McDonell 1997; Turner 2001). Yet the increasing complexity attending the expansion of neoliberal governance and the uneven integration of the global marketplace has driven policymakers and corporate owners alike to rely more on experts (Beck 1992).

(6)

experts' knowledge, nonexpert citizens have both the political leverage and the moral

responsibility to hold experts accountable for the validity of that knowledge (Hartelius 2011:62; Mieg 2001:14; Turner 2001:144 145), which they do by granting or withholding their trust.

However they frame the relationship between nonexperts and experts, studies of this type show how the latter, with its formal knowledge and codified practices (Brand and Karvonen 2007), is usually privileged by technocracy and bureaucracy. Modern expertise in fact has an intimate relationship with the technocratic order (Burris 1989; Topçu 2008), making

nonexpertise an inescapable (if variably designated) role. Nonexperts matter in these studies to the extent that they pose challenges to technocratic management and to experts' professional conduct. Hence the concern exhibited in legal studies (Boccaccini and Brodsky 2002; Mnookin 1999; Tye 1997; T. Ward 2006), medical sociology (Condit and Bates 2005; Gingras 2005; Sanders, Diamond, and Vidmar 2002; Springett 2008; Zadravec, Grad, and So an 2006), and studies of risk and disaster (Drottz-Sjöberg 1991) with how professionals in each field should best deal with nonexperts as clients, opponents, or obstacles. Contrarians in this vein, frequently from natural resource management (Gootee et al. 2010; Reeves 2008) and risk assessment and communication (Hampel 2006; Young and Matthews 2007), discuss turning the liability of dealing with nonexperts into an advantage. Although an increasing number of studies take nonexpert "expertise" seriously as a resource and nonexperts themselves as junior

partners (see medical sociology: e.g., Kerr, Cunningham-Burley, and Amos 1998; Nordin 2000; Popay and G. Williams 1996), the inclusion of nonexperts and their "mental models" for most is explicitly instrumental: to make expert management go more smoothly (Jenkins 2010; Krystallis et al. 2007; Lipworth et al. 2009; Owen et al. 1999).

Foregrounding nonexpertise

(7)

technocratic and academic imaginaries (Blok, Jensen, and Kaltoft 2008; Maranta et al. 2003; Roos 2007). The other is that a good number of scholars are in fact trying to rethink

nonexpertise, even if none of them state it explicitly as their focus. I will review and appraise two promising approaches already evident in the expertise literature. In their own way, and taken collectively, they go some distance towards negating the original negation of sundering expert from nonexpert.1

1. Nonexpertise as knowledge

The first approach derives from a strain of science studies, usually focusing on the relations among scientists and nonscientists in high-stakes public decisions, in which members of the lay public are valorized as capable political actors and possessors of valid knowledge (Healy 2009). Knowledge and skill are distributed unevenly in social life, and not precisely as the

expert/nonexpert boundary-work would suggest (Popay and Williams 1996). Furthermore, the distinctions made between expert and nonexpert, knowledge and ignorance, science and society, are fluid and fragile (Bell and Sheail 2005); once drawn in one context, they don't automatically transfer to others, and frequently have to be redrawn.

This means practically that "realizing one s self as an expert can hinge on casting other people as less aware, knowing, or knowledgeable" (Carr 2010:22). Do nonexperts identify as such by casting other people as more knowledgeable? In many cases, certainly, but not without some ambivalence and contestation. For instance, medical patients (Gülich 2003; I. Shaw 2002) and those at risk from hazards (Burningham, Fielding, and Thrush 2008; Gilgan 2001; Roth 2004; Wisner et al. 2004) may in most things defer to designated experts while still claiming the expertise of knowing their own experiences better than experts can. Other nonexperts go beyond this to make a recognized contribution to experts' understanding of their experiences,

1Unlike either Hegel or Marx and Engels, I don't propose this reframing as some kind of dialectical law of

(8)

even if the experts did not ask for it (Epstein 1995). Still others, like the weather-workers, accept their nonexpertise in science but assert the validity of a whole other epistemological system (Cook 2004), which may not "articulate" either by engaging with larger audiences or by making itself clear in cosmopolitan or technocratic terms (Choy 2005).

Paradoxically this approach seeks to boost the epistemic respectability of nonexperts by playing up the alterity and scientific incommensurability of their knowledge (e.g., Healy 2009)--variously termed "folk epistemology" (Taylor 1992), "civic epistemology" (Wynne 2003), "lay epistemology" (Irwin and Michael 2003), or "street science" (Corburn 2002). Such an

epistemology is portrayed as local, contextual, and practical (Eden 1996; Ricci, Bellaby, and Flynn 2010); as privileging individual subjective experience (Corburn 2002; Cytryn 2001; McClean and A. Shaw 2005; Whelan 2001) and collective identities and traditions (McKechnie 1996; Satterfield 1997; Vouligny, Domon, and Ruiz 2009); and as addressing risks by mixing knowledge, intuition, and moral judgments (Cytryn 2001; Maschio 2007; Neil, Malmfors, and Slovic 1994; Sjöberg 1998; Slimak and Dietz 2006). This presents an interesting resonance with work seeking to validate traditional indigenous knowledge (Leach and Fairhead 2002; Mahiri 1998). Yet where "citizen scientists" attempt to engage conceptually and morally with

universalizing Western science, indigenous knowledge experts generally seek disengagement from science and its experts. (The enthusiastic boundary-work between volcano scientists and ufologists, and the divergence in respect accorded by volcano scientists and weather-workers to each other, seem to support this characterization.)

(9)

as one more human knowledge system (e.g., Adas 1990; Downey and Dumit 1997; Harding 2008; Worseley 1997) as well as efforts in science studies to treat science as one more human activity capable of sociological analysis, albeit an elite (Fuller 2002), esoteric (Collins and Evans 2007; Frank 1949; Hronzsky 2003), or even deviant (Neidhardt 1993) one.

2. Nonexpertise as role-playing

A second approach proceeds from the recognition that "almost anyone can act as an expert"--or, conversely, as a nonexpert--for someone else in an increasingly fragmented and opaque public sphere (Mieg 2006:746). Given this relational and contingent nature, what is necessary to focus on is how actual people achieve and inhabit these roles in the course of situated social interaction (Jacoby and Gonzalez 1991; Matoesian 1999; Mieg 2001; Rifkin and Martin 1997). The question thus becomes why and under what conditions someone would take on the role of nonexpert vis-à-vis someone else.

Since certain types of expertise are only as valid as a nonexpert constituency is prepared to endorse (Hartelius 2011:23 25), taking the nonexpert role can have some significant

(10)

This relational/interactional approach to nonexpertise supports the findings that what qualifies as science, expertise, and even knowledge is in many ways open and contested. The roles themselves can be precarious and contingent, especially in controversial areas (Dew and Lloyd 1997). I experienced just such role-playing under uncertainty when I was invited on a local Puebla variety TV show as an expert on the ethnography of the Popocatépetl region. I found myself sharing the set with the director of the state satellite office, Mexico's most visible ufologist, and a host who tried to create "good TV" by soliciting our comments on photos of mysterious "letters" that had appeared in the snows of Iztaccíhuatl that morning in conjunction with a UFO report. The encounter succinctly recapitulated in real-time the dance claimants of expertise enact over how to interpret phenomena, whose approaches should be respected (exactly how far), and how not to overtly disparage those playing the counterexpert role or alienate their nonexpert audience.

Conclusion

By whichever path we approach the issue of (non)expertise, we ought not to ignore the ways in which inequalities in the ideological arena shape how agentive actors can be in positioning self and other. Some find it easier to argue for (non)expert status due, on the one hand, to

institutional links or their absence--imagine a university geologist disclaiming expert knowledge of Popocatépetl, and then imagine a ufologist doing the same--and, on the other, to intellectual fashions and political sensitivities--few in the volcanic villages would at this point choose to be aligned with "the experts", for example. Nor can we evade the responsibility of dealing

(11)

Works Cited Adas, Michael

1990 Machines as the Measure of Men: Science, Technology, and Ideologies of Western Dominance. Ithaca: Cornell University Press.

Albores, Beatriz, and Johanna Broda, eds.

1997 Graniceros: Cosmovisión y Meteorología Indígenas de Mesoamérica. México DF: Universidad Nacional Autonóma de México.

André, Didier-Jean, and Gobet Fernand

2008 Sherlock Holmes - an expert s view of expertise. British Journal of Psychology 99(1): 109 125.

Ayton, Peter

1992 On the Competence and Incompetence of Experts.InExpertise and Decision Support. George Wright and Fergus Bolger, eds. Pp. 77 105. New York: Springer US.

Baker, Jason K., Daniel S. Messinger, Naomi V. Ekas, Kristin M. Lindahl, and Ryan Brewster 2010 Nonexpert Ratings of Family and Parent Child Interaction. Journal of Family Psychology

24(6):775 778. Beach, Lee Roy

1992 Epistemic Strategies: Causal Thinking in Expert and Nonexpert Judgment.InExpertise and Decision Support. George Wright and Fergus Bolger, eds. Pp. 107 127. New York: Springer. Beck, Ulrich

1992 Risk Society. New Delhi: Sage Publications. Bell, Morag, and John Sheail

2005 Experts, Publics and the Environment in the UK: Twentieth-Century Translations. Journal of Historical Geography 31(3):496 512.

Blok, Anders, Mette Jensen, and Pernille Kaltoft

2008 Social Identities and Risk: Expert and Lay Imaginations on Pesticide Use. Public Understanding of Science 17(2):189 209.

Boccaccini, Marcus T., and Stanley L. Brodsky

2002 Believability of Expert and Lay Witnesses: Implications for Trial Consultation. Professional Psychology: Research and Practice 33(4):384 388.

Brand, Ralf, and Andrew Karvonen

2007 The Ecosystem of Expertise: Complementary Knowledges for Sustainable Development. Sustainability: Science, Practice, and Policy 3(1).

Broda, Johanna, Stanislaw Iwaniszewski, and Arturo Montero, eds.

2001 La Montaña en el Paisaje Ritual. 1st edition. México D.F.: Universidad Nacional Autonóma de México.

Buchanan, Bruce G., Randall Davis, and Edward A. Feigenbaum

2006 Expert Systems: A Perspective from Computer Science.InThe Cambridge Handbook of Expertise and Expert Performance. K. Anders Ericsson, Neil Charness, Paul J. Feltovich, and Robert R. Hoffman, eds. Pp. 87 103. New York: Cambridge University Press.

Burningham, Kate, Jane Fielding, and Diana Thrush

2008 It'll Never Happen to Me : Understanding Public Awareness of Local Flood Risk. Disasters 32(2):216 238.

Burris, Beverly H.

1989 Technocratic Organization and Control. Organization Studies 10(1):1 22. Campbell, Brian

1985 Uncertainty as Symbolic Action in Disputes Among Experts. Social Studies of Science 15: 429 453.

Carr, E. Summerson

2010 Enactments of Expertise. Annual Review of Anthropology 39(1):17 32. Choy, Timothy K.

2005 Articulated Knowledges: Environmental Forms after Universality s Demise. American Anthropologist 107(1):5 18.

Collins, Harry, and Robert Evans

(12)

Condit, Celeste, and Benjamin R. Bates

2005 How Lay People Respond to Messages about Genetics, Health, and Race. Clinical Genetics 68(2):97 105.

Cook, Ryan J.

2004 Weather-workers, Saucer Seekers, and Orthoscientists: Epistemic Authority in Central Mexico. Ph.D. dissertation, Department of Anthropology, University of Chicago.

Corburn, Jason

2002 Street Science: The Fusing of Local and Professional Knowledge in Environmental Policy. Ph.D. dissertation, Massachusetts Institute of Technology.

Cousin, Marie-Eve, and Michael Siegrist

2010 Risk Perception of Mobile Communication: A Mental Models Approach. Journal of Risk Research 13(5):599 620.

Cytryn, Kayla Naomi

2001 Lay Reasoning and Decision-Making Related to Health and Illness. Ph.D. dissertation, McGill University.

Dew, Kevin, and Mike Lloyd

1997 Accounting for the ICI fire: Partial Connections Between Lay and Expert Knowledge. Journal of Sociology 33(3):394 402.

Downey, Gary Lee, and Joseph Dumit, eds.

1997 Cyborgs and Citadels: Anthropological Interventions in Emerging Sciences and Technologies. 1st edition. Santa Fe: School of American Research Press.

Drottz-Sjöberg, Britt-Marie

1991 Non-Expert Definitions of Risk and Risk Perception. Rhizikon--Risk Research Reports, 3. Stockholm: Center for Risk Research, Stockholm School of Economics.

Durant, Darrin

2008 Burying Nuclear Waste, Exposing Nuclear Authority: Canada s Nuclear Waste Disposal Concept and Expert-Lay Discourse. Ph.D. dissertation, University of Toronto.

Eden, Sally

1996 Public Participation in Environmental Policy: Considering Scientific, Counter-Scientific and Non-Scientific Contributions. Public Understanding of Science 5(3):183 204.

Epstein, Steven

1995 The Construction of Lay Expertise: AIDS Activism and the Forging of Credibility in the Reform of Clinical Trials. Science, Technology & Human Values 20(4):408 437.

Ericsson, K. Anders

2006 An Introduction to Cambridge Handbook of Expertise and Expert Performance: Its

Development, Organization, and Content.InThe Cambridge Handbook of Expertise and Expert Performance. K. Anders Ericsson, Neil Charness, Paul J. Feltovich, and Robert R. Hoffman, eds. Pp. 3 19. New York: Cambridge University Press.

Ericsson, K. Anders, Neil Charness, Paul J. Feltovich, and Robert R. Hoffman, eds.

2006 The Cambridge Handbook of Expertise and Expert Performance. Cambridge UK: Cambridge University Press.

Feltovich, Paul J., Kenneth M. Ford, and Robert R. Hoffman, eds.

1997 Expertise in Context: Human and Machine. Menlo Park CA ; Cambridge MA: AAAI Press ; MIT Press.

Ferriz, Pedro, and Cristian Siruguet

1981 Los Hombres del OVNI. México D.F.: Editorial Diana. Fischer, Frank

2003 Expertise as Hermeneutic and Emancipatoric Knowledge.InExpertise and Its Interfaces: The Tense Relationship of Science and Politics. Gotthard Bechmann and Imre Hronzsky, eds. Pp. 45 52. Gesellschaft - Technik - Umwelt, 4. Berlin: Edition Sigma.

Frank, Jerome

1949 The Place of the Expert in a Democratic Society. Philosophy of Science 16(1):3 24. Fuller, Steve

2002 Social Epistemology. 2nd edition. Bloomington: Indiana University Press. Fuller, Steve, and James H. Collier

(13)

Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Gieryn, Thomas

1999 Cultural Boundaries of Science: Credibility on the Line. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Gilgan, Megan

2001 The Rationality of Resistance: Alternatives for Engagement in Complex Emergencies. Disasters 25(1):1 18.

Gingras, Jacqui

2005 Evoking Trust in the Nutrition Counselor: Why Should We Be Trusted? Journal of Agricultural and Environmental Ethics 18(1):57 74.

Glockner, Julio

1996 Los Volcanes Sagrados: Mitos y Rituales en el Popocatépetl y la Iztaccíhuatl. México DF: Grijalbo.

Gootee, Roje S., Keith A. Blatner, David M. Baumgartner, Matthew S. Carroll, and Edward P. Weber 2010 Choosing What to Believe About Forests: Differences Between Professional and

Non-Professional Evaluative Criteria. Small-Scale Forestry 9(2):137 152. Gülich, Elisabeth

2003 Conversational Techniques Used in Transferring Knowledge between Medical Experts and Non-Experts. Discourse Studies 5(2):235 263.

Hampel, Jürgen

2006 Different Concepts of Risk A Challenge for Risk Communication. International Journal of Medical Microbiology 296:5 10.

Hanea, D. M., H. M. Jagtman, L. L. M. M. van Alphen, and B. J. M. Ale

2010 Quantitative and Qualitative Analysis of the Expert and Non-Expert Opinion in Fire Risk in Buildings. Reliability Engineering & System Safety 95(7):729 741.

Harding, Sandra

2008 Sciences From Below: Feminisms, Postcolonialities, and Modernities. Durham: Duke University Press.

Hartelius, E. Johanna

2011 The Rhetoric of Expertise. Lanham MD: Lexington Books. Healy, Stephen

2009 Toward an Epistemology of Public Participation. Journal of Environmental Management 90(4):1644 1654.

Howard, Robert W.

2009 Individual Differences in Expertise Development Over Decades in a Complex Intellectual Domain. Memory & Cognition 37(2):194 209.

Hronzsky, Imre

2003 Scientific Expertise Encounters the Public to Develop a Cooperative Cognition Mode.In

Expertise and Its Interfaces: The Tense Relationship of Science and Politics. Gotthard Bechmann and Imre Hronsky, eds. Pp. 143 184. Gesellschaft - Technik - Umwelt, 4. Berlin: Edition Sigma.

Irwin, Alan, and Mike Michael

2003 Science, social theory and public knowledge. Maidenhead: Open University. Irwin, Alan, and Brian Wynne

1996 Introduction.InMisunderstanding Science? The Public Reconstruction of Science and

Technology. Alan Irwin and Brian Wynne, eds. Pp. 1 17. Cambridge UK: Cambridge University Press.

Jacoby, Sally, and Patrick Gonzalez

1991 The Constitution of Expert-Novice in Scientific Discourse. Issues in Applied Linguistics 2(2):149 181.

Jenkins, Fred

2010 A Study of the Relationship Between Ecological Risk Communication and Lay Public

Participation in Federal Rulemaking Concerning Ecological Risks. Ph.D. dissertation, George Mason University.

Kaufman, James C., John Baer, and Jason C. Cole

(14)

Kerr, Anne, Sarah Cunningham-Burley, and Amanda Amos

1998 The New Genetics and Health: Mobilizing Lay Expertise. Public Understanding of Science 7(1):41 60.

Krystallis, Athanasios, Lynn Frewer, Gene Rowe, Julie Houghton, Olga Kehagia, and Toula Perrea 2007 A Perceptual Divide? Consumer and Expert Attitudes to Food Risk Management in Europe.

Health, Risk & Society 9(4):407 424. Latour, Bruno

1997 Science in Action: How to Follow Scientists and Engineers Through Society. rev. ed. edition. Cambridge MA: Harvard University Press.

Leach, Melissa, and James Fairhead

2002 Manners of Contestation: Citizen Science and Indigenous Knowledge in West Africa and the Caribbean. International Social Science Journal 54(3):299-?

Lee, Harry F., and David D. Zhang

2004 Perceiving Desertification from the Lay Perspective in Northern China. Land Degradation & Development 15(6):529 542.

Lipworth, Wendy L., Heather M. Davey, Stacy M. Carter, Claire Hooker, and Wendy Hu 2009 Beliefs and Beyond: What Can We Learn from Qualitative Studies of Lay People s

Understandings of Cancer Risk? Health Expectations 13(2):113 124. Loudin, Ji i

2003 A Counter-Expert: Cognitive or Social Alternative?InExpertise and Its Interfaces: The Tense Relationship of Science and Politics. Gotthard Bechmann and Imre Hronzsky, eds. Pp. 225 244. Gesellschaft - Technik - Umwelt, 4. Berlin: Edition Sigma.

Macías Medrano, Jesús Manuel, ed.

2005 La Disputa por el Riesgo en el Volcán Popocatépetl. México D.F.: Centro de Investigación y Estudios Superiores en Antropología Social.

Mahiri, Ishmail O.

1998 The Environmental Knowledge Frontier: Transects with Experts and Villagers. Special issue, The 1997 Annual Conference of the Development Studies Association, Journal of International Development 10(4):527 537.

Mann, Derek T. Y., A. Mark Williams, Paul Ward, and Christopher M. Janelle

2007 Perceptual-Cognitive Expertise in Sport: A Meta-Analysis. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology 29:457 478.

Maranta, Alessandro, Michael Guggenheim, Priska Gisler, and Christian Pohl

2003 The Reality of Experts and the Imagined Lay Person. Acta Sociologica 46(2):150 165. Margolis, Howard

1996 Dealing with Risk: Why the Public and the Experts Disagree on Environmental Issues. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

Martínez Jiménez, José Luis

1992 El Maravilloso Mundo de los OVNIs Visto por la Ciencia. Colección Reportaje, 13. México D.F.: La Prensa.

Maschio, Thomas

2007 How Doctors and Patients Talk Past One Another: The Clash of Scientific and Folk Worldviews. Anthropology News 48(4):25 26.

Matoesian, Gregory M.

1999 The Grammaticalization of Participant Roles in the Constitution of Expert Identity. Language in Society 28(4):491 521.

McClean, Stuart, and Alison Shaw

2005 From Schism to Continuum? The Problematic Relationship Between Expert and Lay Knowledge An Exploratory Conceptual Synthesis of Two Qualitative Studies. Qualitative Health Research 15(6):729 749.

McDonell, Gavan

1997 Scientific and Everyday Knowledge: Trust and the Politics of Environmental Initiatives. Social Studies of Science 27(6):819 863.

McKechnie, Rosemary

(15)

126 151. Cambridge UK: Cambridge University Press. Mieg, Harald A.

2001 The Social Psychology of Expertise: Case Studies in Research, Professional Domains, and Expert Roles. Mahwah NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

2006 Social and Sociological Factors in the Development of Expertise.InThe Cambridge Handbook of Expertise and Expert Performance. K. Anders Ericsson, Neil Charness, Paul J. Feltovich, and Robert R. Hoffman, eds. Pp. 743 760. New York: Cambridge University Press.

Mnookin, Jennifer L.

1999 Images of Truth: Evidence, Expertise, and Technologies of Knowledge in the American Courtroom. Ph.D. dissertation, Massachusetts Institute of Technology.

Mohr, Lawrence B.

1994 Authority in Organizations: On the Reconciliation of Democracy and Expertise. Journal of Public Administration Research and Theory: J-PART 4(1):49 65.

Neidhardt, Friedhelm

1993 The Public as a Communication System. Public Understanding of Science 2(4):339 350. Neil, Nancy, Torbjörn Malmfors, and Paul Slovic

1994 Intuitive Toxicology: Expert and Lay Judgments of Chemical Risks. Toxicologic Pathology 22(2):198 201.

Nordin, Ingemar

2000 Expert and Non-expert Knowledge in Medical Practice. Medicine, Health Care and Philosophy 3(3):295 302.

Owen, Amy J., Jeni S. Colbourne, Christopher R. I. Clayton, and Chris Fife-Schaw

1999 Risk Communication of Hazardous Processes Associated with Drinking Water Quality: A Mental Models Approach to Customer Perception, Part 1 - A Methodology. Water Science and

Technology 39(10-11):183 188. Pearson, Thomas

2009 On the Trail of Living Modified Organisms: Environmentalism within and against Neoliberal Order. Cultural Anthropology 24(4):712 745.

Pedersen, Inge Kryger, and Charlotte Baarts

2010 Fantastic Hands But No Evidence: The Construction of Expertise by Users of CAM☆. Social Science & Medicine 71(6):1068 1075.

Pfadenhauer, Michaela

2006 Crisis or Decline? Problems of Legitimation and Loss of Trust in Modern Professionalism. Current Sociology 54(4):565 578.

Philippon, Axelle C., Julie Cherryman, Ray Bull, and Aldert Vrij

2007 Lay People s and Police Officers Attitudes Towards the Usefulness of Perpetrator Voice Identification. Applied Cognitive Psychology 21(1):103 115.

Popay, Jennie, and Gareth Williams

1996 Public Health Research and Lay Knowledge. Social Science & Medicine 42(5):759 768. Prior, Lindsay

2003 Belief, Knowledge and Expertise: The Emergence of the Lay Expert in Medical Sociology. Sociology of Health & Illness 25:41 57.

Randle, Kevin

1999 Scientific Ufology. New York: Avon Books. Reeves, Reginald

2008 GIS Mapping of Two Worlds : Comparing Expert and Non-expert Conservation Priorities. M.S. thesis, College of Charleston.

Reinhard, M. A., K. Burghardt, S. L. Sporer, and S. E. Bursch

2002 Lay Persons Beliefs Regarding Content-Related Cues of Deception. Zeitschrift für Soziologie 33(3):169 180.

Ricci, Miriam, Paul Bellaby, and Rob Flynn

2010 Engaging the Public on Paths to Sustainable Energy: Who Has to Trust Whom? Energy Policy 38(6):2633 2640.

Rifkin, William D., and Brian Martin

(16)

Roos, Michael W. M.

2007 Nonexpert Beliefs about the Macroeconomic Consequences of Economic and Noneconomic Events. Public Choice 132(3/4):291 304.

Roth, Wolff-Michael

2004 Those Who Get Hurt Aren t Always Being Heard: Scientist-Resident Interactions over Community Water. Science, Technology & Human Values 29(2):153 183.

Rouse, Joseph

1996 Engaging Science: How to Understand its Practices Philosophically. Ithaca, N.Y.: Cornell University Press.

Rowe, Gene, and George Wright

2001 Differences in Expert and Lay Judgments of Risk: Myth or Reality? Risk Analysis 21(2): 341 356.

Sánchez-Manzanares, Miriam, Ramón Rico, and Francisco Gil

2008 Designing Organizations: Does Expertise Matter? Journal of Business and Psychology 23 (3-4):87 101.

Sanders, Joseph, Shari S. Diamond, and Neil Vidmar

2002 Legal perceptions of science and expert knowledge. Psychology, Public Policy, and Law 8: 139 153.

Satterfield, Theresa

1997 Voodoo Science and Common Sense: Ways of Knowing Old-Growth Forests. Journal of Anthropological Research 53(4):443 459.

Selinger, Evan M., and Robert P. Crease, eds.

2006 The Philosophy of Expertise. New York: Columbia University Press. Selinger, Evan M., and John Mix

2006 On Interactional Expertise: Pragmatic and Ontological Concerns.InThe Philosophy of Expertise. Evan M. Selinger and Robert P. Crease, eds. Pp. 302 321. New York: Columbia University Press.

Shaw, Ian

2002 How Lay Are Lay Beliefs? Health: An Interdisciplinary Journal for the Social Study of Health, Illness and Medicine 6(3):287 299.

Sheaffer, Robert

1998 UFO Sightings: The Evidence. New York: Prometheus Books. Sjöberg, Lennart

1998 Risk Perception. European Psychologist 3(1):1 12. Slimak, Michael W., and Thomas Dietz

2006 Personal Values, Beliefs, and Ecological Risk Perception. Risk Analysis 26(6):1689 1705. Springett, Jane

2008 Combining Lay Knowledge with Expert Knowledge in Promoting Health: Dialogues in Context. Psychology & Health 23(1):242 243.

Story, Ronald

1980 UFOs and the Limits of Science. New York: Morrow. Stover, Mark

2004 The Reference Librarian as Non-Expert: A Postmodern Approach to Expertise. The Reference Librarian 42(87-88):273 300.

Sturrock, Peter, ed.

2000 The UFO Enigma. New York: Warner Media. Taylor, Charles A.

1992 Of Audience, Expertise, and Authority: The Evolving Creationism Debate. The Quarterly Journal of Speech 78(3):277 295.

Topçu, Sezin

2008 Confronting Nuclear Risks: Counter-Expertise as Politics Within the French Nuclear Energy Debate. Nature and Culture 3:225 245.

Turner, Stephen

2001 What is the Problem with Experts? Social Studies of Science 31(1):123 149. Tye, Marcus Choi

(17)

Statements. Ph.D. dissertation, The University of North Dakota. Vouligny, Évelyne, Gérald Domon, and Julie Ruiz

2009 An Assessment of Ordinary Landscapes by an Expert and by its Residents: Landscape Values in Areas of Intensive Agricultural Use. Land Use Policy 26(4):890 900.

Ward, Tony

2006 English Law s Epistemology of Expert Testimony. Journal of Law and Society 33(4):572 595. Webster, Kevin, Cindy Jardine, Sean B. Cash, and Lynn M. McMullen

2010 Risk Ranking: Investigating Expert and Public Differences in Evaluating Food Safety Hazards. Journal of Food Protection 73(10):1875 1885.

Whelan, Emma C.

2001 Well Now, Who s the Doctor Here? : Boundary-work and Transgression in Patient and Expert Knowledges of Endometriosis. Ph.D. dissertation, Carleton University.

Wisner, Benjamin, Piers Blaikie, Terry Cannon, and Ian Davis

2004 At Risk: Natural Hazards, People s Vulnerability, and Disasters. 2nd edition. London: Routledge. Worseley, Peter

1997 Knowledges: Culture, Counterculture, Subculture. New York: New Press. Wright, George, Fergus Bolger, and Gene Rowe

2002 An Empirical Test of the Relative Validity of Expert and Lay Judgments of Risk. Risk Analysis 22(6):1107 1122.

Wright, George, A. Pearman, and K. Yardley

2000 Risk Perception in the UK Oil and Gas Production Industry: Are Expert Loss-prevention Managers Perceptions Different from Those of Members of the Public? Risk Analysis 20(5):681 690.

Wynne, Brian

2003 Seasick on the Third Wave? Subverting the Hegemony of Propositionalism: Response to Collins & Evans. Social Studies of Science 33(3):401 417.

Young, Nathan, and Ralph Matthews

2007 Experts Understanding of the Public: Knowledge Control in a Risk Controversy. Public Understanding of Science 16(2):123 144.

Zadravec, Tina, Onja Grad, and Gregor So an

Referensi

Dokumen terkait

Sehubungan dengan tahap evaluasi dan pembuktian kualifikasi dalam proses Pekerjaan Pemeliharaan Kendaraan Dinas Operasional, dengan ini kami mengundang Saudara untuk

Pada hari ini, Rabu tanggal Dua Puluh Dua Bulan Pebruari Tahun Dua Ribu Dua Belas, Panitia Pengadaan Barang / Jasa Dilingkungan Bidang Bina Marga Dinas Pekerjaan Umum

sekedar sebagai pengajar semata, pendidik akademis tetapi juga merupakan pendidik karakter, moral dan budaya bagi siswanya. Maka dari itu pendidikan karakter sangatlah

Tips yang mudah dalam mendekorasi kamar tidur klasik dan berikut kami akan berikan beberapa Desain Kamar Set Klasik Terbaru 2017.. Kamar set mewah terbaru dengan desain klasik saat

- Penyusunan dan Updating Data Perkembangan Tanaman Pangan, Horikultura dan Perkebunan. Lokasi Kegiatan

Hasil korelasi antara kualitas pelayanan dokumen dan kecepatan bongkar muat general cargo secara bersama-sama dengan kepuasan pengguna jasa diperoleh r sebesar 0,814 yang artinya

Pengaruh Deskripsi Pekerjaan dan Koordinasi Terhadap Efektivitas Kerja Karyawan PT PLN (Persero) Cabang Medan. Pengaruh Pembagian Kerja Terhadap Efektivitas Karyawan

Dimulai dari tambah jenis paket,kode paket,simpan data,rercord duplikat,pesan data duplikat.untuk menghapus paket dimulai dari tampilan halaman utama,hapus,selanjutnya hapus